Measuring bias in movement perception Our measure of visual perception (that could be adapted by movement practice) is the bias to see ambiguous apparent.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Ago, ergo Cogito Language comprehension warps the mirror neuron system Arthur M. Glenberg Arizona State University University of Wisconsin - Madison September,
Advertisements

Chapter 4: The Visual Cortex and Beyond
Andreas Kleinschmidt INSERM U992 CEA NeuroSpin Saclay, France Mind Reading - Can Imaging Tell What You Are Thinking?
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
Attentionally Dependent Bilateral Advantage on Numerosity Judgments Jenny Ewing & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Mirror Neurons.
Pre-frontal cortex and Executive Function Squire et al Ch 52.
Autism And Mirror Neurons. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Affected people exhibit a wide range in the magnitude in their symptoms These patients show.
Prior observation or production of a motor action improves the perception of biological motion: Evidence for a gender effect Christel Ildéi-Bidet 1, Alan.
Neural Correlates of Visual Awareness. A Hard Problem Are all organisms conscious?
Lesions of Retinostriate Pathway Lesions (usually due to stroke) cause a region of blindness called a scotoma Identified using perimetry note macular sparing.
Test on Friday!. Lesions of Retinostriate Pathway Lesions (usually due to stroke) cause a region of blindness called a scotoma Identified using perimetry.
Haptic Signals for Communication under Workload In a primarily visual task, haptic signals can be more resistant to large cognitive workloads than visual.
Mirror Neurons.
From Perception to Action And what’s in between?.
Writing Workshop Find the relevant literature –Use the review journals as a first approach e.g. Nature Reviews Neuroscience Trends in Neuroscience Trends.
Searching for the NCC We can measure all sorts of neural correlates of these processes…so we can see the neural correlates of consciousness right? So what’s.
Post-test review session Tuesday Nov in TH241.
Post-test review session Tuesday Nov in TH241.
Final Review Session Neural Correlates of Visual Awareness Mirror Neurons
Motion and Ambiguity Russ DuBois. Ambiguity = the possibility to interpret a stimulus in two or more ways Q: Can motion play a part in our interpretation.
Visual Imagery One of the greatest problems confronting psychology is the nature of mental representation. Part of this debate is the nature of representations.
Visual Cognition I basic processes. What is perception good for? We often receive incomplete information through our senses. Information can be highly.
Motor cortex Organization of motor cortex Motor cortical map Effect of cortical motor neuron activation on muscle contraction Population coding.
DDMLab – September 27, ACT-R models of training Cleotilde Gonzalez and Brad Best Dynamic Decision Making Laboratory (DDMLab)
Motor cortical areas: the homunculus The motor system.
Motor cortical areas: the homunculus The motor system.
 The misinformation effect refers to incorrect recall or source attribution of an item presented after a to-be-remembered event as having been presented.
Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System MELVYN A. GOODALE* AND ANGELA HAFFENDEN Department of Psychology, University of.
Percevoir l'imperceptible par l'action : l'exemple du mouvement biologique Christel Ildéi-Bidet, Alan Chauvin & Yann Coello Université Charles De Gaulle,
Intention Detection and Mirror Neurons
Studying Memory Encoding with fMRI Event-related vs. Blocked Designs Aneta Kielar.
The effect of varying the visual context on trajectory planning and perceptual awareness of one’s own performance 1 Psychology and Neurocognition Laboratory.
Understanding Action Verbs- Embodied Verbal Semantics Approach Pavan Kumar Srungaram M.Phil Cognitive Science (09CCHL02) Supervisor: Prof. Bapi.
Bayesian goal inference in action observation by co-operating agents EU-IST-FP6 Proj. nr Raymond H. Cuijpers Project: Joint-Action Science and.
Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion
Chapter 3: Neural Processing and Perception. Neural Processing and Perception Neural processing is the interaction of signals in many neurons.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Give examples of the way that virtual reality can be used in Psychology.
An Eyetracking Analysis of the Effect of Prior Comparison on Analogical Mapping Catherine A. Clement, Eastern Kentucky University Carrie Harris, Tara Weatherholt,
RIGHT PARIETAL CORTEX PLAYS A CRITICAL ROLE IN CHANGE BLINDNESS by Naser Aljundi.
Dialog Processing with Unsupervised Artificial Neural Networks Andrew Richardson Thomas Jefferson High School for Science and Technology Computer Systems.
Orienting Attention to Semantic Categories T Cristescu, JT Devlin, AC Nobre Dept. Experimental Psychology and FMRIB Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford,
Effect of laterality-specific training on visual learning Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Corresponding author: Ruth Raymaekers, Ghent University, Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Research Group Developmental Disorders;
C.E.R.M.A.C. Centro Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo XIX SYMPOSIUM NEURORADIOLOGICUMBOLOGNA – 4th-9th OCTOBER 2010 THE SHAPE OF MOTOR RESONANCE:
 In a vacuum, perception should evolve to be increasingly accurate  Selection pressure on deception  Thus, pressure to detect deception  Adaptive.
The Development of Infants’ Sensitivity to the Orientation of Object Motion: Predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick.
Chapter 11 Language. Some Questions to Consider How do we understand individual words, and how are words combined to create sentences? How can we understand.
Oliver Sawi1,2, Hunter Johnson1, Kenneth Paap1;
Experimental Design in Functional Neuroimaging
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
WELLCOME DEPARTMENT OF IMAGING NEUROSCIENCE
Volume 21, Issue 2, Pages (January 2011)
Intrinsic and Task-Evoked Network Architectures of the Human Brain
Wallis, JD Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute UC, Berkeley
Redmond G. O’Connell, Michael N. Shadlen, KongFatt Wong-Lin, Simon P
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Beth L. Parkin, Hamed Ekhtiari, Vincent F. Walsh  Neuron 
Stan Van Pelt and W. Pieter Medendorp
Conserved Sequence Processing in Primate Frontal Cortex
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
Presentation transcript:

Measuring bias in movement perception Our measure of visual perception (that could be adapted by movement practice) is the bias to see ambiguous apparent movement toward the observer or away from the observer. The staircase procedure used to measure the bias was developed by Lewis & McBeath (2004). Participants view on a computer monitor a screen tiled with stimuli that appear to recede into the distance (Figure 2). Figure 2. The tiling for one stimulus, the bean hand A second screen frame is presented one second later with shape tiles slightly shifted, which induces an illusion of motion that can be in either depth direction. A shift of 50% of the inter-tile distance is completely ambiguous, whereas a shift of 20% in either direction is perceived as movement in the smaller direction of shift. We start with a shift of 20% and then small increments in shift are added until the observer perceives movement in the opposite direction (a turn point). The increment is then reduced and the turn point measured in the opposite direction. Two sets of eight turn points are collected, and the averages of the last seven (in each set) are used as the measure of the threshold, or bias, to perceive movement in one direction or another. A behavioral method for studying mirror neurons: Repetitive action affects visual perception Arthur M. Glenberg 1,2, Gabriel Lopez-Mobilia 1, Michael McBeath 1, Michael Toma 1, Marc Sato 3, and Luigi Cattaneo 4 1 Arizona State University, 2 University of Wisconsin - Madison, 3 University of Grenoble, 4 University of Trento Abstract Mirror neurons may underlie the ability to make sensorimotor predictions when observing action, and thus contribute to “reading” intentions of other animals and facilitating social interaction. Neurophysiological and brain imaging studies have shown that observation of both biological and non- biological movements activates a fronto-parietal network of motor regions which forms the core of the human mirror-neuron system (MNS). However, many past findings are intrinsically correlational. We developed a behavioral method to study mirror neurons, based on use-induced plasticity. Participants engage in a repetitive motor task of moving beans from one location to another, thereby adapting the neural systems used in control of the action. Participants then engage in a second task, which measures if performance is affected by the motor adaptation. In the current study, the second task measures perceptual bias of ambiguous apparent motion in one direction or another. We found that the direction of bean movement (toward or away from the participant) systematically increases the bias to experience ambiguous movement in the opposite direction (consistent with habituation). The results corroborate previous findings in which the second tasks were language-based The findings support the claim that a MNS is being tapped, and confirm functionality of our method for studying the causal contribution of MNS to cognitive processes. Why not fMRI? 1.Because the fMRI voxel is sensitive to activity from many neurons, simply showing that an area is active both during action and perception does not guarantee that the same neurons are responding to both action and perception (e.g., Dinstein et al, 2007). 2.In most fMRI experiments, the signal only indicates a correlation, not a causal or functional relation. 3.fMRI is difficult to use with children. 4.It’s expensive. Use-induced plasticity Repetitive practice of a movement can produce a temporary re-organization of the brain. For example, Classen et al. (1998) used a TMS pulse over motor cortex controlling the thumb and elicited a movement in a particular direction. Then participants practiced moving the thumb in a different direction for about 20 minutes. Finally, TMS over the same area of motor cortex now tended to elicit movement in the practiced direction. Use-induced plasticity and mirror neurons: The logic Mirror neurons play a role in both producing action and perceiving action. Thus, if practice adapts part of a mirror neuron system, then the effects of that practice should be revealed in a perceptual task that uses the same mirror neuron system. In our experiments, we rule out non-specific effects of practice by adapting the motor system using two opposing practice directions. Showing different effects of the direction of practice on visual perception rules out non-specific (e.g., fatigue) effects. The bi-directional action task Participants move beans, one at a time, from a wide-mouth container to a narrow-mouth container an arms-length away (Figure 1.) Half of the participants move the beans in a direction Away from the self, and half move the beans in a direction Toward the self. Note that very similar muscles are used in the two conditions. Thus the actions are defined in terms of target location and specifics of the movement (e.g., a power grip is used in the Away movement but not the return toward the body, and vice versa in the Toward movement). Figure 1. Bean containers illustrating the Away condition. Experiment 1 Procedure 1.Bias in motion perception was measured for the three stimulus shapes: a hand holding a bean, an open hand with palm up, and a plain diamond (see Figure 3a). The three stimuli were used to determine if we are tapping “strictly-congruent” (only the hand stimulus should show evidence of change in bias) or “broadly-congruent” mirror neurons (all directional stimuli may show a change in bias). The type of stimulus was manipulated within-subjects, and the tests of bias for each of the three stimuli were interleaved so that a participant might see a diamond shift, a hand holding a bean shift, and then an open hand shift. 2.Next, 48 participants moved 600 beans in one direction or another. The movement direction was manipulated between-subjects. 3.Last, bias in motion perception was measured again for the three stimuli. Results Figure 3 (right) shows the change in measured bias as a function of bean movement direction on the second, post-movement, measure of bias. The interaction of bean movement direction and pre-movement or post- movement measure of bias was significant, F(1, 46) = 5.16, p=.03. Figure 3. Stimuli (left) & Results (right) for Exp. 1. Conclusions 1.Direction of action (bean movement) systematically affects visual perception of ambiguous apparent motion in aligned directions. Findings support the claim that we tapped into a mirror neuron system. 2.The mirror neuron response is “broadly-congruent” with the same effect found for all three stimuli (all exhibited a similar habituation bias). Experiment 2 We made several changes in the methodology to rule out alternative explanations. Most importantly, participants were blindfolded during the bean movement task. Thus, any affect of movement on the bias measure can be more securely attributed to adapting an action system rather than visual stimulation. Second, to eliminate any sensitization to toward and away movement due to the initial bias measure, the 24 participants first moved 300 beans in one direction, and then we measured bias. Participants then moved an additional 300 beans in the same direction, and the bias was measured again. The data for the first, post-movement bias measure are in Figure 4. The effect of bean movement direction was significant, F(1,22) = 5.89, p =.02. The effects were not significant when measured after the second set of bean movements. Figure 4. Experiment 2: Results for the first, post-movement bias measure. Error bars are one standard error. A = bean movement away from the body; T = bean movement toward the body. Plasticity and language comprehension Glenberg, Sato, and Cattaneo (2008) used the same bean task as in Experiment 1. Participants then read and responded to sentences describing action Toward the participant (e.g., “Art gives the pen to you”) or Away from the participant (e. g., “You give the pen to Art”). The response was whether the sentence was sensible or nonsense (e.g., “You give Art to the pen”). Participants indicated sensible by depressing a key with the right index finger, and they indicated nonsense by depressing a key with the left index finger. Neither response required arm movements. The findings indicated an interaction such that the time to respond sensible to Toward sentences was faster when the beans were moved in the away direction and the time respond sensible to the Away sentences was faster when the beans were moved in the toward direction. Interestingly, the same effects were found for sentences describing the transfer of concrete objects and sentences describing the transfer of abstractions (e.g., “Anna delegates the responsibilities to you”). Apparently, neural systems used in action control were also used for comprehending sentences describing actions of the same general sort. Plasticity and speech perception Sato, Brisebois, Grabski, Basirat, Menard, Glenberg, & Cattaneo (2008) had participants purse their lips 150 times to induce changes in corticomotor control of the orofacial musculature. Subsequently, they performed a speeded two-choice identification task of acoustically presented /pa/ and /ta/ CV syllables either embedded in acoustic noise or not. A control task was identical but without the lip pursing. They observed a decrease in “pa” responses in the control session and overall faster RTs in the motor session. References Casile A., Giese, M.A. (2006) Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 69–74. Classen, J., Liepert, J., Wise, S.P., Hallett, M., Cohen, L.G. (1998). Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. J. Neurophysiol. 79, Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Behrmann, M. and Heeger, D. (2008) A mirror up to nature. Current Biology, 18, 1, R Di Pellegrino G., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Gallese V., Rizzolatti G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp. Brain Res. 91, 176–80. Gallese V., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Rizzolatti G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. Glenberg, A.M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L. (2008). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18, R1-R2. Lewis C.F., McBeath M.K. (2004). Bias to experience approaching motion in a three-dimensional virtual environment. Perception, 33, Rizzolatti, G., Craighero, L. (2004). The Mirror-Neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, Rizzolatti G., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, Sato, M., Brisebois, A., Grabski, K., Basirat, A, Ménard, L, Glenberg, A. M., & Cattaneo, L. (October, 2008). Paper presented at the Speech and Face to Face Communication Workshop, Grenoble, France. Author Note: This research was support in part by NSF grant BCS Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. Direct correspondence to Arthur Glenberg, Conclusions Our technique appears to have adapted a neural system tuned to multiple modalities including motor, visual, and language processes. Although more research is needed, so far these results are consistent with claims that a human mirror neuron system exists and that it contributes to action perception, speech perception, and language comprehension. Furthermore, this simple method provides researchers with a promising instrument to study the role of mirror neuron systems in these and other cognitive processes without the expense and inconvenience of fMRI. Ago, ergo Cogito Two-frame apparent motion tasks show promise as a hit with some observers!