Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Based upon classic decision puzzlers collected by Gretchen.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter Nineteen The American Economy Personal Finances ~~~~~ Insurance Against Hardship.
Advertisements

Post Research Benefits Mandika Wijeyaratne MS, MD, FRCS Dept. of Surgery, Colombo.
Health and Human Sciences Economics and Health: a taster Masters in Public Health Key reference: McPake B., Kumaranayake, L. & Normand, C (2002) Health.
An Exploration of Decision Processes in an Evolutionary Perspective: the Case of the Framing Effect.
Shana Schwarz.  Type I (i.e. false positive) ◦ Occurs by rejecting the null hypothesis even when it is true. ◦ In other words, this is the error of accepting.
1 Intuitive Irrationality: Reasons for Unreason. 2 Epistemology Branch of philosophy focused on how people acquire knowledge about the world Descriptive.
Thinking: Concept Formation Concept formation: identifying commonalities across stimuli that unite them into a common category Rule learning: identifying.
Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Based upon classic decision puzzlers collected by Gretchen.
Survey.
Extension Article by Dr Tim Kenny
Decision making and economics. Economic theories Economic theories provide normative standards Expected value Expected utility Specialized branches like.
Running Experiments with Amazon Mechanical-Turk Gabriele Paolacci, Jesse Chandler, Jesse Chandler Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 5, No. 5, August 2010.
Behavioral Economics Chapter 30. What Is Behavioral Economics? The study of choices actually made by economic decision makers in an effort to assess the.
Or Why We’re Not Really As Rational As We’d Like to Believe.
Statistics for the Social Sciences Psychology 340 Fall 2006 Hypothesis testing.
EXTRA PRACTICE WITH ANSWERS
Chapter 12 Sample Surveys
Using Market Research. Seeing the Problem Clearly Marketing Research: procedure designed to identify solutions to a specific marketing problem through.
Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Based upon classic decision puzzlers collected by Gretchen.
Study Design Population Sampling Frame Interview Length
Math 227 Elementary Statistics
Modular 15 Ch 10.1 to 10.2 Part I. Ch 10.1 The Language of Hypothesis Testing Objective A : Set up a Hypothesis Testing Objective B : Type I or Type II.
A Summary Of Key Findings From A National Survey Of Voters. #07160.
Hypothesis Testing A hypothesis is a conjecture about a population. Typically, these hypotheses will be stated in terms of a parameter such as  (mean)
The smokers’ proportion in H.K. is 40%. How to testify this claim ?
Testing a Claim I’m a great free-throw shooter!. Significance Tests A significance test is a formal procedure for comparing observed data with a claim.
Copyright © 2000 by Harcourt, Inc. All rights reserved. What is Perception? Perception: The process of recognizing and understanding others By understanding.
Testing Hypotheses Tuesday, October 28. Objectives: Understand the logic of hypothesis testing and following related concepts Sidedness of a test (left-,
Review of Related Literature Different decision-making: – Budget decisions of managers – Irrationality of continuing the risk of losing a prospect – Decision-making.
Decision making Making decisions Optimal decisions Violations of rationality.
The State of Arthritis in Michigan 2005 Michigan Department of Community Health Chronic Disease Epidemiology Section, Division of Genomics, Perinatal.
Framing Effects From Chapter 34 ‘Frame and Reality’ of Thinking Fast and Slow, by D. Kahneman.
§ 1.1 An Overview of Statistics. Data and Statistics Data consists of information coming from observations, counts, measurements, or responses. Statistics.
SOME QUESTIONS???? PAPIA BAWA Class Exercise. Question Set 1 Suppose we are trying to gather statistics regarding the efficacy of the death penalty. Here.
Next Previous view Unit 6 Next Previous People predict by things about the a. knowing.... past- b. thinking..... present c. saying.....
An Overview of Statistics
Mohammad Aljawadi PharmD, PhD Clinical Pharmacy Department King Saud University PHCL 431 Sep, 2015.
Figure 1. Distribution of Individuals Covered by Private Health Insurance, by Type of Health Plan Comprehensive = health plan with no deductible or
Don’t cry because it is all over, smile because it happened.
Why is Research Important?. Basic Research Pure science or research Research for the sake of finding new information and expanding the knowledge base.
Lecture 15 – Decision making 1 Decision making occurs when you have several alternatives and you choose among them. There are two characteristics of good.
Introduction Hypothesis testing for one mean Hypothesis testing for one proportion Hypothesis testing for two mean (difference) Hypothesis testing for.
Warm Up 2/20/2014. Principles of Experimental Design (CRR) 1)Control the effects of lurking variables on the response, most simply by comparing.
Reframe the problem or the solution
Research Strategies. Why is Research Important? Answer in complete sentences in your bell work spiral. Discuss the consequences of good or poor research.
Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Based upon classic decision puzzlers collected by Gretchen.
Welcome to MM570 Psychological Statistics
14 Statistical Testing of Differences and Relationships.
Business Statistics, A First Course (4e) © 2006 Prentice-Hall, Inc. Chap 9-1 Chapter 9 Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests Business Statistics,
Psychology As Science Psychologists use the “scientific method” Steps to the scientific method: - make observations - ask question - develop hypothesis.
Survey Research And a few words about elite interviewing.
Copyright © 2013, 2009, and 2007, Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 10 Comparing Two Groups Section 10.1 Categorical Response: Comparing Two Proportions.
1 DECISION MAKING Suppose your patient (from the Brazilian rainforest) has tested positive for a rare but serious disease. Treatment exists but is risky.
TODAY’S GOALS Conduct beginning of class debates.
Organization of statistical investigation. Medical Statistics Commonly the word statistics means the arranging of data into charts, tables, and graphs.
Hypothesis Testing Introduction to Statistics Chapter 8 Feb 24-26, 2009 Classes #12-13.
Causal inferences This week we have been discussing ways to make inferences about the causal relationships between variables. One of the strongest ways.
1 BAMS 517 – 2011 Decision Analysis -IV Utility Failures and Prospect Theory Martin L. Puterman UBC Sauder School of Business Winter Term
Adverse Selection. What Is Adverse Selection Adverse selection in health insurance exists when you know more about your likely use of health services.
Introduction to Hypothesis Testing Chapter 10. What is a Hypothesis? A tentative explanation for an observation, phenomenon, or scientific problem that.
Jump to first page 1 Models of Health Promotion n Stages of Change, Prochaska et al (1982) n Health Belief Model, Rosenstock (1974)
The Foreign Language Effect on The Sunk-Cost Fallacy
Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences
Unit 5: Hypothesis Testing
Effects of Foreign Language on Decision Making
Everybody’s Talking… Welcome to the Safe Harbor Term LIFE Insurance
DIS 280 Social Science Research Methodology: Problem Framing
Significance Tests: The Basics
Significance Tests: The Basics
Presentation transcript:

Rationality Alan Kaylor Cline Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Based upon classic decision puzzlers collected by Gretchen Chapman of Rutgers University

1. Sunk Cost: Group A is told: As the president of a large pharmaceutical company, you have invested 10 million dollars of the company's money into a research project. The purpose was to develop a vaccine that would prevent people from acquiring HIV. When the project is 90% completed, another firm begins marketing a vaccine that prevents HIV infection. Also, it is apparent that their vaccine is more effective and less expensive than the vaccine your company is developing. The question is: should you invest the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to finish your HIV vaccine?

1. Sunk Cost: Group A is told: As the president of a large pharmaceutical company, you have invested 10 million dollars of the company's money into a research project. The purpose was to develop a vaccine that would prevent people from acquiring HIV. When the project is 90% completed, another firm begins marketing a vaccine that prevents HIV infection. Also, it is apparent that their vaccine is more effective and less expensive than the vaccine your company is developing. The question is: should you invest the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to finish your HIV vaccine? The results are: Yes (63%) and No (37%)

1. Sunk Cost: Group B is told: As the president of a large pharmaceutical company, you have received a suggestion from one of your employees. The suggestion is to use the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to develop a vaccine that would prevent people from acquiring HIV. However, another firm has just begun marketing a vaccine that prevents HIV infection. Also, it is apparent that their vaccine is more effective and less expensive than the vaccine your company could develop. The question is: should you invest the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to develop the proposed HIV vaccine?

1. Sunk Cost: Group B is told: As the president of a large pharmaceutical company, you have received a suggestion from one of your employees. The suggestion is to use the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to develop a vaccine that would prevent people from acquiring HIV. However, another firm has just begun marketing a vaccine that prevents HIV infection. Also, it is apparent that their vaccine is more effective and less expensive than the vaccine your company could develop. The question is: should you invest the last 1 million dollars of your research funds to develop the proposed HIV vaccine? The results are: Yes (27%) and No (73%)

1. Sunk Cost: For Group B the results are: Yes (27%) and No (73%) For Group A the results are: Yes (63%) and No (37%) But the situation for A is very similar to that for B

1. Sunk Cost: For Group B the results are: Yes (27%) and No (73%) For Group A the results are: Yes (63%) and No (37%) But the situation for A is very similar to that for B For more info., see: Arkes, H.R. & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. OBHDP, 35,

2. Conjunction Fallacy: A health survey was conducted in a representative sample of adult males in Chicago of all ages and occupations. Mr. F was included in the sample. He was selected by random chance from the list of participants. Please rank the following statements in terms of which is most likely to be true of Mr. F. (1=more likely to be true, 6=least likely) 1. ____ Mr. F smokes more than 1 cigarette per day on average. 2. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks. 3. ____ Mr. F had a flu shot this year. 4. ____ Mr. F eats red meat at least once per week. 5. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks and he is over 55 years old. 6. ____ Mr. F never flosses his teeth.

2. Conjunction Fallacy: A health survey was conducted in a representative sample of adult males in Chicago of all ages and occupations. Mr. F was included in the sample. He was selected by random chance from the list of participants. Please rank the following statements in terms of which is most likely to be true of Mr. F. (1=more likely to be true, 6=least likely) 1. ____ Mr. F smokes more than 1 cigarette per day on average. 2. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks. 3. ____ Mr. F had a flu shot this year. 4. ____ Mr. F eats red meat at least once per week. 5. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks and he is over 55 years old. 6. ____ Mr. F never flosses his teeth. Choice 5 includes choice 2, yet 21% rated “5” more likely than “2”. Everyone should rank “2” more likely than “”5”.

2. Conjunction Fallacy: A health survey was conducted in a representative sample of adult males in Chicago of all ages and occupations. Mr. F was included in the sample. He was selected by random chance from the list of participants. Please rank the following statements in terms of which is most likely to be true of Mr. F. (1=more likely to be true, 6=least likely) 1. ____ Mr. F smokes more than 1 cigarette per day on average. 2. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks. 3. ____ Mr. F had a flu shot this year. 4. ____ Mr. F eats red meat at least once per week. 5. ____ Mr. F has had one or more heart attacks and he is over 55 years old. 6. ____ Mr. F never flosses his teeth. Choice 5 includes choice 2, yet 21% rated “5” more likely than “2”. Everyone should rank “2” more likely than “”5”. For more information, see: Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90,

3. Omission Bias: In the state where you live, there have been several epidemics of a certain kind of flu, which can be fatal to children under 3. The probability of each child getting the flu is 1 in 10, but only 1 in 100 children who get the flu will die from it. This means 10 out of 10,000 children will die. A vaccine for this kind of flu has been developed and tested. The vaccine eliminates the probability of getting the flu. The vaccine, however, might cause side effects that are also sometimes fatal. The children who die from the side effects of the vaccination are not necessarily the same ones who would die from the flu. Imagine that you are married and have one child, a one-year old. You wonder whether you should vaccinate your child. Your child will have a 10 in 10,000 chance of dying from the flu without vaccination. Would you vaccinate your child if the overall death rate for vaccinated children were (check those in which you would vaccinate): ____ 0 in 10,000____ 6 in 10,000 ____ 1 in 10,000____ 7 in 10,000 ____ 2 in 10,000____ 8 in 10,000 ____ 3 in 10,000____ 9 in 10,000 ____ 4 in 10,000____ 10 in 10,000 ____ 5 in 10,000

3. Omission Bias: Imagine that you are married and have one child, a one-year old. You wonder whether you should vaccinate your child. Your child will have a 10 in 10,000 chance of dying from the flu without vaccination. Would you vaccinate your child if the overall death rate for vaccinated children were: 8% 0 in 10,0004% 6 in 10,000 2% 1 in 10,0000% 7 in 10,000 2% 2 in 10,0000% 8 in 10,000 2% 3 in 10,00047% 9 in 10,000 0% 4 in 10,00015% 10 in 10,000 21% 5 in 10,000

3. Omission Bias: For more information, see: Ritov, I. & Baron, J. (1990). Reluctance to vaccinate: Omission bias and ambiguity. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 3,

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for outbreak of an unusual disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the program are as follows:

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for outbreak of an unusual disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the program are as follows: Group A is told: If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved. If Program B is adopted, there is a one-third probability that 600 people will be saved and a two-thirds probability that no people will be saved.

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for outbreak of an unusual disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the program are as follows: Group A is told: If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved. 67% If Program B is adopted, there is a one-third probability that 600 people will be saved and a two-thirds probability that no people will be saved. 33%

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for outbreak of an unusual disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the program are as follows: Group B is told: If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die. If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die.

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for outbreak of an unusual disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the program are as follows: Group B is told: If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die. 8% If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die. 92%

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Group B is told: If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die. 8% If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die. 92% Group A is told: If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved. 67% If Program B is adopted, there is a one-third probability that 600 people will be saved and a two-thirds probability that no people will be saved. 33%

4. Reflection Framing Effect: Group B is told: If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die. 8% If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die. 92% Group A is told: If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved. 67% If Program B is adopted, there is a one-third probability that 600 people will be saved and a two-thirds probability that no people will be saved. 33% The results are almost inverted even though Program A for Group A is logically equal to Program C for Group B and Program B for Group A is logically equal to Program D for Group B.

4. Reflection Framing Effect: For more information, see: Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39,

5. Multiple Alternatives: The patient is a 67-year-old farmer with chronic right hip pain. The diagnosis is osteoarthritis. You have tried several nonsteroidal anti- inflammatory agents (e.g., aspirin, naproxen, and ketoprofen) and have stopped them because of either adverse effects or lack of efficacy. You decide to refer him to an orthopedic consultant for consideration for hip replacement surgery. The patient agrees to this plan.

5. Multiple Alternatives: The patient is a 67-year-old farmer with chronic right hip pain. The diagnosis is osteoarthritis. You have tried several nonsteroidal anti- inflammatory agents (e.g., aspirin, naproxen, and ketoprofen) and have stopped them because of either adverse effects or lack of efficacy. You decide to refer him to an orthopedic consultant for consideration for hip replacement surgery. The patient agrees to this plan. Group A: Before sending him away, however, you check the drug formulary and find that there is one nonsteroidal medication that this patient has not tried (ibuprofen). What do you do? 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 2. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication.

5. Multiple Alternatives: The patient is a 67-year-old farmer with chronic right hip pain. The diagnosis is osteoarthritis. You have tried several nonsteroidal anti- inflammatory agents (e.g., aspirin, naproxen, and ketoprofen) and have stopped them because of either adverse effects or lack of efficacy. You decide to refer him to an orthopedic consultant for consideration for hip replacement surgery. The patient agrees to this plan. Group A: Before sending him away, however, you check the drug formulary and find that there is one nonsteroidal medication that this patient has not tried (ibuprofen). What do you do? 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 89% 2. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication. 11%

5. Multiple Alternatives: The patient is a 67-year-old farmer with chronic right hip pain. The diagnosis is osteoarthritis. You have tried several nonsteroidal anti- inflammatory agents (e.g., aspirin, naproxen, and ketoprofen) and have stopped them because of either adverse effects or lack of efficacy. You decide to refer him to an orthopedic consultant for consideration for hip replacement surgery. The patient agrees to this plan. Group B: Before sending him away, however, you check the drug formulary and find that there is one nonsteroidal medication that this patient has not tried (ibuprofen). What do you do? 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 35% 2. Refer to orthopedics and also start piroxicam. 4% 3. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication. 58%

5. Multiple Alternatives: Group B: 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 35% 2. Refer to orthopedics and also start piroxicam. 4% 3. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication. 58% Before sending him away, however, you check the drug formulary and find that there is one nonsteroidal medication that this patient has not tried (ibuprofen). What do you do? Group A: 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 89% 3. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication. 11%

5. Multiple Alternatives: The patient is a 67-year-old farmer with chronic right hip pain. The diagnosis is osteoarthritis. You have tried several nonsteroidal anti- inflammatory agents (e.g., aspirin, naproxen, and ketoprofen) and have stopped them because of either adverse effects or lack of efficacy. You decide to refer him to an orthopedic consultant for consideration for hip replacement surgery. The patient agrees to this plan. Group B: Before sending him away, however, you check the drug formulary and find that there is one nonsteroidal medication that this patient has not tried (ibuprofen). What do you do? 1. Refer to orthopedics and also start ibuprofen. 35% 2. Refer to orthopedics and also start piroxicam. 4% 3. Refer to orthopedics and do not start any new medication. 58% But option 2 in for Group A is the same as option 3 Group B, thus we conclude that discovery of a new drug may lead to worse patient care.

5. Multiple Alternatives: For more information, see: Redelmeier, D.A. & Shafir, E. (1995). Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. JAMA, 273(4),

6. Attraction effect: Imagine that one of your patients suffers from migraine headaches that last about 3 hours and involve intense pain, nausea, dizziness, and hyper-sensitivity to bright lights and loud noises. The patient usually needs to lie quietly in a dark room until the headache passes. Out of every 365 days (1 year), this patient has a migraine headache on about 100 of those days (8.3 per month). Of course, on a day when the patient has a headache, she doesn't spend the entire day in pain, but only about 3 hours of that day. You are considering three medications that you could prescribe for this patient. All three medications have only negligible side effects, and any side effects are the same for the three. Each medication comes in the form of pills that must be taken once per day. The medications differ in effectiveness and cost. The patient has a low income and must pay the cost because her insurance plan does not cover any of these medications. And of course the patient is also the one who appreciates the effectiveness.

6. Attraction effect: Imagine that one of your patients suffers from migraine headaches that last about 3 hours and involve intense pain, nausea, dizziness, and hyper-sensitivity to bright lights and loud noises. The patient usually needs to lie quietly in a dark room until the headache passes. Out of every 365 days (1 year), this patient has a migraine headache on about 100 of those days (8.3 per month). Of course, on a day when the patient has a headache, she doesn't spend the entire day in pain, but only about 3 hours of that day. You are considering three medications that you could prescribe for this patient. All three medications have only negligible side effects, and any side effects are the same for the three. Each medication comes in the form of pills that must be taken once per day. The medications differ in effectiveness and cost. The patient has a low income and must pay the cost because her insurance plan does not cover any of these medications. And of course the patient is also the one who appreciates the effectiveness. Group A: Three options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. Drug C: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 60 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year.

6. Attraction effect: Imagine that one of your patients suffers from migraine headaches that last about 3 hours and involve intense pain, nausea, dizziness, and hyper-sensitivity to bright lights and loud noises. The patient usually needs to lie quietly in a dark room until the headache passes. Out of every 365 days (1 year), this patient has a migraine headache on about 100 of those days (8.3 per month). Of course, on a day when the patient has a headache, she doesn't spend the entire day in pain, but only about 3 hours of that day. You are considering three medications that you could prescribe for this patient. All three medications have only negligible side effects, and any side effects are the same for the three. Each medication comes in the form of pills that must be taken once per day. The medications differ in effectiveness and cost. The patient has a low income and must pay the cost because her insurance plan does not cover any of these medications. And of course the patient is also the one who appreciates the effectiveness. Group A: Three options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 11% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 88% Drug C: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 60 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 0%

6. Attraction effect: Imagine that one of your patients suffers from migraine headaches that last about 3 hours and involve intense pain, nausea, dizziness, and hyper-sensitivity to bright lights and loud noises. The patient usually needs to lie quietly in a dark room until the headache passes. Out of every 365 days (1 year), this patient has a migraine headache on about 100 of those days (8.3 per month). Of course, on a day when the patient has a headache, she doesn't spend the entire day in pain, but only about 3 hours of that day. You are considering three medications that you could prescribe for this patient. All three medications have only negligible side effects, and any side effects are the same for the three. Each medication comes in the form of pills that must be taken once per day. The medications differ in effectiveness and cost. The patient has a low income and must pay the cost because her insurance plan does not cover any of these medications. And of course the patient is also the one who appreciates the effectiveness. Group B: Two options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year.

6. Attraction effect: Imagine that one of your patients suffers from migraine headaches that last about 3 hours and involve intense pain, nausea, dizziness, and hyper-sensitivity to bright lights and loud noises. The patient usually needs to lie quietly in a dark room until the headache passes. Out of every 365 days (1 year), this patient has a migraine headache on about 100 of those days (8.3 per month). Of course, on a day when the patient has a headache, she doesn't spend the entire day in pain, but only about 3 hours of that day. You are considering three medications that you could prescribe for this patient. All three medications have only negligible side effects, and any side effects are the same for the three. Each medication comes in the form of pills that must be taken once per day. The medications differ in effectiveness and cost. The patient has a low income and must pay the cost because her insurance plan does not cover any of these medications. And of course the patient is also the one who appreciates the effectiveness. Group B: Two options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 35% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 65%

6. Attraction effect: Group B: Two options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 35% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 65% Group A: Three options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 11% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 88% Drug C: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 60 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 0%

6. Attraction effect: Group B: Two options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 35% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 65% Group A: Three options Drug A: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 30 days with a headache per year. It costs $350 per year. 11% Drug B: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 50 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 88% Drug C: reduces the number of headaches from 100 days with a headache per year to 60 days with a headache per year. It costs $100 per year. 0% But the information about Drugs A and B in the three option version is the same as the information about Drugs A and B in the two option version. The addition of the Drug C, although chosen by no one, has increased the selection of Drug B.

6. Attraction effect: For more information, see: Huber, J., Payne, J.W. & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 90-98; and Chapman, G.B. & Malik, M.M. (1995). The attraction effect in prescribing decisions and consumer choice. Medical Decision Making, 15, 414.

7. Outcome bias: A 55-year-old man had a heart condition. He had to stop working because of chest pain. He enjoyed his work and did not want to stop. His pain also interfered with other things, such as travel and recreation. A type of bypass operation would relieve his pain and increase his life expectancy from age 65 to age 70. However, 8% of the people who have this operation die from the operation itself. His physician decided to go ahead with the operation. 1. The physician who made the decision first consulted the patient. The patient could not decide and asked the physician's advice. The physician knew that the patient would accept this advice. Hence, it is the physician who makes the decision on the patient's behalf. 2. The physician who made the decision is not the one who carried out the procedure. 3. The physician who made the decision had no more relevant information than you are given, and there is no more relevant information that can be discovered. Evaluate the physician's decision to go ahead with the operation (circle one): 3=clearly correct, and the opposite decision would be inexcusable 2=correct, all things considered 1=correct, but the opposite would be reasonable too 0=decision and its opposite are equally good -1=incorrect, but not unreasonable -2=incorrect, all things considered -3=incorrect and inexcusable

7. Outcome bias: Group A: A 55-year-old man had a heart condition. He had to stop working because of chest pain. He enjoyed his work and did not want to stop. His pain also interfered with other things, such as travel and recreation. A type of bypass operation would relieve his pain and increase his life expectancy from age 65 to age 70. However, 8% of the people who have this operation die from the operation itself. His physician decided to go ahead with the operation. The operation succeeded.

7. Outcome bias: Group A: A 55-year-old man had a heart condition. He had to stop working because of chest pain. He enjoyed his work and did not want to stop. His pain also interfered with other things, such as travel and recreation. A type of bypass operation would relieve his pain and increase his life expectancy from age 65 to age 70. However, 8% of the people who have this operation die from the operation itself. His physician decided to go ahead with the operation. The operation succeeded. The mean score was 1.52

7. Outcome bias: Group A: A 55-year-old man had a heart condition. He had to stop working because of chest pain. He enjoyed his work and did not want to stop. His pain also interfered with other things, such as travel and recreation. A type of bypass operation would relieve his pain and increase his life expectancy from age 65 to age 70. However, 8% of the people who have this operation die from the operation itself. His physician decided to go ahead with the operation. The operation failed and the man died.. The mean score was 1.57

7. Outcome bias: A 55-year-old man had a heart condition. He had to stop working because of chest pain. He enjoyed his work and did not want to stop. His pain also interfered with other things, such as travel and recreation. A type of bypass operation would relieve his pain and increase his life expectancy from age 65 to age 70. However, 8% of the people who have this operation die from the operation itself. His physician decided to go ahead with the operation. The operation succeeded. or The operation failed and the man died. The operation succeeded : The mean score was.85 The operation failed and the man died: The mean score was Thus the advice, that had to given before the operation, was declared to be correct or incorrect based upon the result.

7. Outcome bias: For more information see: Baron, J. and Hershey, J.C. (1988). Outcome bias in decision evaluation. JPSP, 54,