To Students in Psychology 293-01 --- Some of you have used part of at least one Trinity Day learning about color perception on your own. The internet is.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Detecting and Mixing Colors STEM DIGITAL Institute Rob Snyder.
Advertisements

OpenGL Course Notes Chapter 5: Lighting Jim Mims, Spring 2009.
Lecture 16 – Lightness Perception
Book Scanning & Digital Image Production The VRC Guide to Imaging By Kate Stepp.
The Ray Box: Part Two. Law of Refraction The law of refraction for light states that a light ray is bent away from its original direction when it passes.
Basic Table-top SEM imaging
Color Vision. Wavelength properties: ● Hue: psychological reaction to different wavelengths of light. (Basically the same thing as color). ● Different.
Unit 4: Sensation & Perception
Light and Color. Light interacting with matter When light hits matter, at least one of three things can happen: – Reflection When light bounces off an.
HUE Hue is color.. Hue Hue is the name of a distinct color of the spectrum—red, green, yellow, orange, blue, etc. It refers to the particular wavelength.
Two equally true statements: The only thing we can hear is sound. The only thing we can see is light.
Colour Theory and Application B.Sc. (Hons) Multimedia ComputingMedia Technologies.
Lecture 30: Light, color, and reflectance CS4670: Computer Vision Noah Snavely.
1 Lecture 6 Attributes of graphical primitives Colors Colors in OpenGL Algorithms for scan-conversion.
Color, lightness & brightness Lavanya Sharan February 7, 2011.
PRINCIPLES OF DESIGN PHOTOGRAPHY. BALANCE Visual center is above geometric center. Visual weight is determined by many variables Size Darkness – A strong.
© Maths Support Service 2007 Binary and Hexadecimal Numbers Next Slide AE98FD AE98FD.
Computer Vision Spring ,-685 Instructor: S. Narasimhan Wean 5403 T-R 3:00pm – 4:20pm.
Design Elements Form & Value To understand and apply the design elements Form & Value.
Guilford County SciVis V104.02
How do we perceive colour? How do colours add?. What is colour? Light comes in many “colours”. Light is an electromagnetic wave. Each “colour” is created.
Elements of Art Line Shape Form Space Value Color Texture.
Product Design Sketching Chromatic Theories. Color Spectrum The range of colors seen by human eye is the “visible color spectrum”
Physics Talk 5.10 Making Different Lights and Paints
Color Correct and Remove Keystoning A minimalist approach to photographing your art By Paul Marley.
Chapter 3 Space. Three Kinds of Space Space as format: size, scale, and presentation. Space as the relationships among objects and the areas surrounding.
CS1315: Introduction to Media Computation Picture encoding and manipulation.
CS1315: Introduction to Media Computation Picture encoding and manipulation.
The color wheel. Eighteenth century scientist Isaac Newton first used a circle to show the colors or hues, of the spectrum. The color wheel was developed.
Perceiving and Recognizing Objects 4. Object Recognition Objects in the brain Extrastriate cortex: The region of cortex bordering the primary visual cortex.
OPTICAL ILLUSIONS AND BRAIN FUNCTION Click on mouse to advance There are no circles here, is your brain fooled?
Vision: change, lightness, acuity
Learning Targets: Review art element “value” Understand Color Theory and Mixing.
Illusions By Linda Doan. The Hermann Square Do you see gray circles in the intersection of the squares? Look again, do those circles really exist?
Color Why is the Sky Blue? Atmosphere scatters blue light because of the shorter λ.
1. 2 What do these things have in common?
Chapter 6 Section 2: Vision. What we See Stimulus is light –Visible light comes from sun, stars, light bulbs, & is reflected off objects –Travels in the.
CS1315: Introduction to Media Computation Picture encoding and manipulation.
CSE 381 – Advanced Game Programming GLSL Lighting.
The Art of Graphic Design
An Introduction to Analyzing Colors in a Digital Photograph Rob Snyder.
INTD 53 dimension of hue.
- Discuss and review the Law of Reflection and mixing of colored light - Investigate the range of visibility of a mirror and practice light ray diagrams.
Creating a Silhouette in Illustrator. Go File>Place and place the photo on the artboard. Select the photo and click Live Trace (its on the tool bar right.
Lecture 34: Light, color, and reflectance CS4670 / 5670: Computer Vision Noah Snavely.
Lighting a 3D Scene Guilford County Sci Vis V part 1.
The Science of Color The How and Why of Color. Color Basics Sir Isaac Newton discovered that white light breaks into a rainbow of colors in 1666 by passing.
Perceptual Constancy Module 19. Perceptual Constancy Perceiving objects as stable or constant –having consistent lightness, color, shape, and size even.
Shade & Shadow Figure 1-3, Page 11
Intelligent Robotics Today: Vision & Time & Space Complexity.
Because you know who said let there be light…. According to this image, Squares A and B are the same shade of gray. True or False?
22.3 Using Color pp Mr. Richter. Agenda  Warm-Up  Review HW  Notes:  How We See Objects  Mixing Pigments (The Subtractive Color Process)
Color.
Understanding Color Correction Outline  Color Casts Defined  Channels Defined  Auto Methods of Color Correction-- Levels  Manual Methods of Color Correction--
Motion and Phases of the Moon The Moon appears to rise in the East and set in the West. This apparent motion of the moon is caused by the rotation.
Light Into Video U7oOI.
Color Mixing light. Misconceptions and problems Mixing light can get very confusing to many students because most have never done it. We have mixed pigments.
Image Processing Intro2CS – week 6 1. Image Processing Many devices now have cameras on them Lots of image data recorded for computers to process. But.
Digital Cameras in the Classroom Day One Basics Ann Howden UEN Professional Development
Binocular Vision Concepts and Examples Size is calibrated without switching into slide show mode. In slideshow mode, the images are a little bit larger.
How Did They Do That? Some secrets you can use to fool the eye. Lessons by Mary Erickson, Ph.D. with art teacher Hillary Andrelchik Masters of Illusion:
Color Mrs. Gergel.
Lesson 2-3 AP Computer Science Principles
Waves Teaching Slides, 3.3 If you are using the PowerPoint slides, log in to your FOSSweb account on your internet browser. Then, cut and paste these links.
FLIPPED CLASS ROOM ACTIVITY CONSTRUCTOR-USING EXISTING CONTENT
Development of CD album booklet
Investigating Color In the Classroom
Colors Computers build colors from Red, Green, and Blue; not Red, Blue, and Yellow. RGB = Red Green Blue Creating Colors Red + Blue = Purple No Red, No.
Light Intro Video
40. Silhouette Illusion Can you mentally make her spin in the other direction? Silhouettes are ambiguous. They can turn either way and it would look exactly.
Presentation transcript:

To Students in Psychology Some of you have used part of at least one Trinity Day learning about color perception on your own. The internet is filled with questions and commentary about the colors of a dress that was accidentally photographed to create an illusion. In the photograph, a deep blue dress with black trim looks like a white dress with gold trim. Or not. There’s a lot of Beau Lotto showmanship in the discussion, meaning that there is some misdirection of attention in order to emphasize the “gee whiz” experience. What you would like to see would be a full video, moving around a little to see how this worked. At least Beau Lotto included video of himself standing up and walking around to show you that the “illusion” only occurred from a special point of view. If you are reading this as a pdf and want to manipulate the images as suggested, then ask me for the PowerPoint version.

If you are one of the few people who have not looked at this photo, here are two versions, an original and a variant that I made by removing all of the background. For me, removing the background did not change very much.

Dress image 1 Dress image 2 -- background darkened

On the next slide is the “true color” of the dress.

This may look the same to you as the first two images. Or it may not. The first two images may switch on you. This one should not.

Remember that I said there was some misdirection going on. The misdirection is concentrating on the color of the dress without talking about the lighting conditions. If you see the dress as white and gold, then you ALSO are seeing it as in shadow. If you see the dress as deep blue with black trim, then you are seeing the lighting conditions as “normal.” On January 26 in the lecture portion of our class, I gave you this Purves figure in PowerPoint so that you could manipulate it. The gray square in the upper left exactly matches the lighted dark stripe AND the shadowed white stripe.

Here is a version of a very famous demonstration by Ted Adelson [Edward H. Adelson] of MIT. Adelson and Alan Gilchrist of Rutgers are two people who study these effects most. These two squares are identical in terms of computer pixel values.

In the “real world,” which we have not arrived at systematically in the course yet, the wavelength and intensity of the light to the eye comes from two main factors, the illuminating light [called the incident light ] and the surfaces the light reflects off of [the reflected light]. In the light ray as such there is no way to tell how much of its quality is caused by the surface color and how much is caused by the illumination quality. Both have played a role. [Compare the role of wavelength and intensity in bleaching the photopigment in a receptor]. Nevertheless, under most conditions, we can sort out these factors very well. To do this, requires sufficiently rich patterning all around. In the absence of enough patterning, lighting can be taken as reflectance and reflectance as lighting – or they can be ambiguous, and hence reversible. Again, in the case of the dress image, the picture had an ambiguity in what was due to the colors of the materials and what was due to the lighting. But they go together, so a change in the appearance of the lighting (shadow or not?) must be accompanied by a change in the apparent colors of the material, because the pixel colors are a product of both.

If the variety of what you can see around you is insufficient, then the surface colors and illumination colors may not be so separable. A sharp spotlight is illumination and does not change the color of the surfaces it shines on. But in isolation, you may not see it this way. Something whose surfaces are black, seen in bright light with nothing close by for comparison will look white. A bright, sharply defined light shined on a black wall can look like a white spot of paint on the wall and not a light. There are two ways to darken an area that you can see: 1. By coloring it darker or 2. By shadowing it. If you see an area darkened by shadow, then you can see the colored material is not changing. This is called “lightness constancy.” In the Adelson display, you see lightness constancy in the checkerboard pattern. The darkening caused by shadowing is not seen as having an effect on the checkerboard colors.

Back to the dress colors Again, notice that if you see the dress as white and gold, you also see it as in shadow. If you see it as deep blue and black, then you do not see it as in shadow. If you can see it either way, then the factors that control which way you see it are a different matter. But if you switch how you see the dress, you switch between possibilities that are equally specified. I doubt that you can see this as a white and gold dress in bright light or a blue and black dress in shadow.

Here is a version of Dress Image 1 that I manipulated in Photoshop. I turned up the contrast and turned down the brightness. Notice that the dress does look blue and it does NOT look like it is in shadow. You see some shadows but they are cast shadows of one part of the dress blocking light to another part.

If you want to explore the effects of some color change here on the computer, try this. [Use PowerPoint version] Select the square and change colors like we did in the rgb class exercise. Start with black and then add red and green (yellow) until you get a dark gold. Note the values of red and green. Then start the square at pure blue ( ) and add the same amount of red and green that you found made the gold. That should give you a kind of purplish color that is a little like the shadowed white. That would mimic the effects of illumination in the sense that uniform illumination has the same effect on all the lightnesses. You should see that the effect on black is similar to the effect on blue.

Summary: It is very straightforward to understand why there are two ways to see the dress. The lighting conditions are ambiguous, and the perceived colors of the dress (the reflectances) go with the lighting conditions. A on the scene could have been made to show the lighting conditions less ambiguously (like Beau Lotto standing up). It is not so straightforward to understand why one sees it one way at one time and the other way at another time. Or why some people see it one way and other people see it another way. Or why some people are very flexible or even able to control the switching. Or whether or not those questions will lead to stable insights. As with all ambiguous figures, one part of understanding the situation is to understand exactly what alternatives are allowed by the geometry. After that, explaining which alternative one sees at a given time, is another matter. The classic ambiguous figure is the Necker cube, on the left. The face that you see as closest to you can switch. Blinking is one way to get it to change. The main point is that the geometry constrains you to two alternatives, but they are equally well defined, so you can see it either way. Seeing it as an elephant battling a hippo is NOT one of the alternatives.

Final note All the time I worked on this PowerPoint account, I saw the dress only as white and gold -- in shadow. Just as I was closing up and saving the final files, I saw the original images as a blue dress with black trim. And I can’t see it any other way now. It really is a reversible figure.

Addendum The following cut-outs from the original image can help you address the question: How much of the photo is necessary to get the effect? If we can find some segments that work, and some segments that do not, it can help us understand what aspects of the original photo are necessary to get the effect.