Protective Effects of Language Development Among Children in Head Start: A Person-Centered Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Introduction to the Environment Rating Scales
Advertisements

Judith J. Carta, Ph.D. Juniper Gardens Children’s Project University of Kansas with help from Elaine Carlson, WESTAT.
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Large databases vs. individual analysis: Two complimentary approaches in the study of education and learning Esther Adi-Japha School of Education, Bar-Ilan.
Massachusetts Early Care and Education and School Readiness Study
The Achievement Gap: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) Tamara Halle, Nicole Forry, Elizabeth Hair & Kate Perper.
Carl E. Bentelspacher, Ph.D., Department of Social Work Lori Ann Campbell, Ph.D., Department of Sociology Michael Leber Department of Sociology Southern.
© 2008 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. 1 CHAPTER 5 Sociocultural Diversity.
Associations between Child Care Quality & Developmental Outcomes of Children in Low Income Working Families in Four Communities James Elicker, Soo-Young.
1 WELL-BEING AND ADJUSTMENT OF SPONSORED AGING IMMIGRANTS Shireen Surood, PhD Supervisor, Research & Evaluation Information & Evaluation Services Addiction.
Women, Minorities, and Technology Jacquelynne Eccles (PI), Pamela Davis-Kean (co-PI), and Oksana Malanchuk University of Michigan.
Childcare Quality and Early Learning Gail E. Joseph, Ph. D
© CCSR Stacy B. Ehrlich, Julia Gwynne, Amber Stitziel Pareja, and Elaine M. Allensworth with Paul Moore, Sanja Jagesic, and Elizabeth Sorice University.
The Targeted Reading Intervention: How Early Reading Intervention for Rural Kindergarten and First-Grade Students Affects Teachers’ Ratings of Students’
School Readiness Initiatives: Assessing Their Yield Craig T. Ramey, Ph.D. & Sharon L. Ramey, Ph.D. Georgetown University Center on Health and Education.
Stability of Resilience in Children of Adolescent Mothers Keri Weed University of South Carolina Aiken Deborah Keogh, and John Borkowski University of.
Cognitive development among young children in Cambodia: Implications for ECED programs.
March 2010 what the school readiness data mean for Harford County’s children ©
The Learning Behaviors Scale
1 Preschoolers Identified as Having Autism: Characteristics, Services, and Achievement Elaine Carlson and Amy Shimshak, Westat OSEP National Early Childhood.
Links to Positive Parenting among African American and Hispanic American Low-Income Mothers Laura D. Pittman Psychology Department Northern Illinois University.
Children and Families in Diverse Settings Margaret Burchinal University of California-Irvine (UNC)
Birth Cohort Jennifer Park National Center for Education Statistics Institute of Education Sciences IES Research Conference June 2006.
1 Quality of Play: Progress and Challenges. 2 Introduction Key Sure Start objective: Improving the ability to learn Core Sure Start service: Support for.
Many children with speech-language impairment will have difficulty with reading. Even those children who begin kindergarten with adequate early literacy.
Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Adults Reading to Two Year Old Children: A Population-based Study Olivia Sappenfield Emory University School of Public Health.
Practitioner Support: Indirect Language Stimulation Techniques to Promote English Language Development in Preschool Students Research Team: Drs. Carolyn.
Foundations and Best Practices in Early Childhood Education: History, Theories and Approaches to Learning, 2 nd Edition © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
Maternal Romantic Relationship Quality, Parenting Stress and Child Outcomes: A Mediational Model Christine R. Keeports, Nicole J. Holmberg, & Laura D.
+ Third Party Evaluation – Interim Report Presentation for Early Childhood Advisory Council December 19, 2013.
A Picture of Young Children in the U.S. Jerry West, Ph.D. National Center for Education Statistics Institute of Education Sciences EDUCATION SUMMIT ON.
Longitudinal Links between Neighborhood Problems, Collective Efficacy, and Adolescents’ Academic and Socioemotional Outcomes Shay M. Galto, Danielle M.
V v References 1. Ladd, G. W., Birch, H. S., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children's Social and Scholastic Lives in Kindergarten: Related Spheres of Influence?
V v Relations between Parental Marital Status, Residential Mobility, and Children’s Academic Achievement and Self-Regulation in Kindergarten Aya Bukres,
Understanding Quality in Early Childhood Education and Care Programs in Massachusetts Findings from the Preschool Program Quality Study and Highlights.
Section 1. Introduction Orientation to Virginia’s QRIS.
F UNCTIONAL L IMITATIONS IN C ANCER S URVIVORS A MONG E LDERLY M EDICARE B ENEFICIARIES Prachi P. Chavan, MD, MPH Epidemiology PhD Student Xinhua Yu MD.
Adult Child Caregivers’ Health Trajectories and Multiple Roles Over Time Amanda E. Barnett, Ph.D. Human Development and Family Studies University of Wisconsin-Stout.
Why should you care about diversity?. 2 There are significant disparities in the education, economic well- being, and health of children in the U.S. based.
Integrated Mother-Premature Infant Intervention and Mother-Infant Interaction at 6-weeks Corrected Age Rosemary White-Traut, PhD, RN, FAAN Kathleen Norr,
INTRODUCTION Maternal and paternal depression are associated with childhood externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. Few studies have examined.
General and Feeding Specific Behavior Problems in a Community Sample of Children Amy J. Majewski, Kathryn S. Holman & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.
Rationale for Inclusion Legal Mandates Head Start Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Americans with Disabilities Act Benefits for children with.
Ready At Five & Maryland State Department of Education.
Sociocultural Influences Related to Language, Cognition, and Social Emotional Developmental Relationships Presenters: Kimberly Sharkins & Dr. James Ernest.
Child Motivation, Shared Book Reading, and Vocabulary Development: A Growth Mixture Modeling Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University.
Psychometric Evaluation of an Instrument for Assessing Policy Outcomes for Families with Children Who Have Severe Developmental Disabilities: The Beach.
Introduction Results: Moderated Mediation The Immigrant Paradox: Associations Between Latino Kindergartners’ Reading and Mathematics Skills, Parents’ Knowledge.
Our Future: Kindergarten Readiness Study for the Le Sueur-Henderson School District Layne Wilbright Minnesota State University, Mankato November 15, 2014.
Using Self-Regulation Research to Inform Intervention Strategies for Latino Preschoolers to Enhance School Readiness Tatiana Nogueira Peredo a, Margaret.
Reciprocal Relations Between Parent-Child Relationship Quality and Children's Adjustment During Early Childhood Chelsea M. Weaver, Anne M. Gill, Katelyn.
Development of Physical Aggression: Exploring the Relationship with Language Elizabeth Anson MS Kimberly Sidora-Arcoleo PhD Robert Cole PhD Harriet Kitzman.
Research Lab for Resilience and Early Childhood Development
Evaluation of Indiana's Pilot Public Pre-Kindergarten Program: First Year Results James Elicker, Katrina Schmerold, David Purpura, Sara Schmitt, Amy Napoli,
Indiana Paths to QUALITY™ Child Care Quality Rating and Improvement System: Outcomes for Children and Child Care Providers James Elicker, Zachary Gold,
Diversity and ECE.
Texas Pediatric Society Electronic Poster Contest
Child Caregiver Interaction Scale (CCIS)
Mothers' Vocabulary and Autonomy-Granting Behaviors as Predictors of Gains in Children's Vocabulary Competence from Age 3 to Age 4 Sara L. Sohr-Preston.
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
The Relationship between Fidelity of Implementation and Classroom Quality in Early Childhood Education Katerina Sergi, M.A.1, Giorgio Carlo Cappello, Ph.D.1,
Testing the Attachment Theory of Parent-Child Interaction Therapy Erin Floyd and Sheila Eyberg Department of Clinical and Health Psychology University.
Attachment Dependency
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Inequality Starts Before Kindergarten
General Social Competence (18)
Child Care and Young Children’s Development
Study Design/Methods Used
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Session V: Environment Rating Scales
Presentation transcript:

Protective Effects of Language Development Among Children in Head Start: A Person-Centered Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University of Wisconsin-Madison INTRODUCTION Children in Head Start programs who come from economically disadvantaged families are likely to lag behind in language development when they enter formal schooling (e.g., Brody, Stoneman, & McCoy, 1994). Research has used the variable-centered approach, rather than the person-centered approach, to identify risk factors (e.g., poverty) and protective factors (e.g., preschool program quality) for these children. A study (Booth Kreisman, 2003) that used the person-centered approach identified heterogeneous literacy developmental trajectories among children in Head Start programs, but it was unclear whether family factors, child factors, and Head Start classroom factors would serve as protective factors to differentiate these developmental patterns. Identifying the protective factors that help children develop language skills in Head Start programs is crucial because intervention programs may promote these factors for low-performing children. This study used the person-centered approach to examine (1) Head Start children’s patterns of vocabulary developmental trajectories, and (2) whether family literacy environment, child motivation, and Head Start classroom quality as protective factors could differentiate the patterns of vocabulary developmental trajectories among children in Head Start programs. RESULTS The growth mixture modeling results showed two distinct patterns of language developmental trajectories: a low- performing group and a decrease-increase group whose language skills decreased in the first year in the Head Start programs and then increased thereafter. The multinomial logistic regression results showed that family literacy environment (shared book reading and the amount of reading materials at home) was a significant predictor of the vocabulary class membership. Children with higher levels of persistence were more likely to be in the decrease-increase group. Children had teachers who encouraged independence were more likely to be in the decrease-increase group. Finally, children in a classroom with more social interactions were likely to be in the decrease-increase group. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to understand (1) the number of vocabulary class among Head Start children; and (3) whether family literacy environment, children’s motivation and persistence, and classroom process quality would be predictive of the vocabulary class membership. Overall, this study demonstrates a number of significant direct paths between positive parenting and home aggression and social skills. A number of control variables were also significantly related to home aggression, social skills, and school aggression. The results underscored the importance of family literacy environment, child persistence, the teachers’ autonomy support, and social interactions in a classroom. These factors can support the children in Head Start programs to develop vocabulary skills by exposing them with literacy materials at home, teaching them the approaches toward learning, fostering positive teacher-student relationships, and creating social classroom environments. DATASET The data used for this project came from the Head Start Family and Child Experiences Survey (FACES—2003 Cohort). The FACES is a national longitudinal study that examines the cognitive, emotional, social, and physical development of the Head Start children in the United States. The FACES study also includes variables that examine the characteristics and well-being of families, the observed quality of Head Start classrooms, and the characteristics and opinions of Head Start teachers and other program staff. The FACES included a sample of 1,778 children who were three and four years old and their parents in a stratified national probability sample of 43 Head Start programs. The sample was stratified by three variables: region of the country, urbanicity, and percentage of minority families in the program. The following Head Start programs were excluded: Migrant and Seasonal Head Start programs, American Indian/Alaska Native Head Start programs, Early Head Start programs, programs in the United States territories, and programs that do not serve children directly. The children were assessed at the initial entry of the Head Start programs (Fall 2003). They were then assessed twice during the Head Start programs (Spring 2004 and Spring 2005). The children were last assessed during their first year in the kindergartens (Spring 2005/2006). REFERENCES Arnett, J. (1989). Caregivers in day-care centers: Does training matter? Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, Booth Kreisman, M. (2003). Evaluating academic outcomes of head start: An application of general growth mixture modeling. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, doi: /s (03) Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & McCoy, J. K. (1994). Contributions of protective and risk factors to literacy and socioemotional competency in former Head Start children attending kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, doi: / (94) Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition. Examiner’s manual and norms booklet. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Harms, T., Clifford, R., & Cryer, D. (1998). Early childhood environment rating scale. New York: Teachers College Press. McDermott, P. A., Green, L. F., Francis, J. M., & Stott, D. H. (2000). Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale. Philadelphia: Edumetric and Clinical Science. Data Sources *The Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, the Administration for Children and Families, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services *Child Care & Early Education Research Connections ABSTRACT This study examined whether the family literacy environment, children’s characteristics, and classroom environment would function as protective factors against the negative effect of poverty on language development among Head Start children. Growth mixture modeling was used to address the research questions. The results showed two classes of vocabulary development: a high-performing class and a low-performing class. Results showed children with parents who frequently read to them and provided reading materials at home were more likely to belong to the resilient group. Children with higher levels of persistence and had teachers who encouraged independence were more likely to be in the resilient group. Finally, children in a classroom with more social interactions were likely to be in the resilient group. METHOD SAMPLE The sample included a total of 2,612 children and their parents or the primary caregivers (mean annual family income = $16,457.75, SD = 12, ). The family structure was diverse (48% two-parent household, 47% mother-only single-parent household, 2% father-only single-parent household, and 3% had neither a mother nor a father as the head of the household). The children’s mean age at program entry was four years old (49% male, 51% female; 29% Caucasian, 31% African American, 33% Hispanic, 7% others). The mothers’ mean age in fall 2003 was years old (SD = 6.24) with 67% having more than high school education. The fathers’ mean age in fall 2003 was years old (SD = 7.38) with 64% having more than high school education. The parents had diverse race and ethnicity backgrounds (mothers: 34% Caucasian, 30% African American, 32% Hispanics, and 4% others; fathers: 30% Caucasian, 34% African American, 31% Hispanics, 5% others). MEASURES The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), the English vocabulary test, was used to assess children’s vocabulary for the four measurement occasions (Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The FACES 2003 adapted a shortened version of the PPVT-III based on Item Response Theory (IRT) analyses. The shortened version had 48 questions. Each question asked the children to look at a series of pictures. The children were asked to point to the picture that corresponded to the word. The questions became increasingly difficult. The reliabilities of the shortened version of the PPVT-III ranged between.84 and.91 across the four measurement occasions. Home literacy environment was measured by shared book reading and the amount of reading materials at home. Shared book reading in fall 2003 was measured by parental report on the frequency of reading to their child in the past week. The rating scale ranged from 0 (not at all in last week) to 7 (daily). Parents also reported the amount of reading materials at home in fall Parents were asked whether they had the following reading materials at home: comic books, magazines for children, magazines for adults, newspapers, catalogs, religious books, and dictionaries or encyclopedias. The parents responded the seven questions with yes (coded as 1) and no (coded as 0). The questions were summed, so that higher scores represented more print exposure at home. The reliability for the amount of reading materials at home was.53. The teacher-student relationship in fall 2003 was measured by the Arnett Caregiver Interaction Scale to assess five areas of teacher behavior: sensitivity, harshness, detachment, permissiveness, and independence with 30 items (Arnett, 1989). Classroom environment was measured by the revised Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale (ECERS-R) in fall 2003 (Harms, Clifford, & Cryer, 1998). Process-related classroom quality was rated by an independent observer with 37 questions on the scale of 1 (inadequate) to 7 (excellent). The subscales of the ECERS-R in the areas of language skills (e.g., providing books) and social skills (e.g., staff-child interactions) were used. Children’s motivation in fall 2003 was measured by the subscales of the Preschool Learning Behavior Scale, competence motivation and persistence (McDermott, Green, Francis, & Stott, 2000). Child gender, family poverty status, mother education, child ethnicity, and mother marital status were included as control variables. ANALYSIS The analyses involved two steps. First, this study used growth mixture modeling (GMM) implemented in Mplus version 6.12 to statistically identify latent trajectory classes of vocabulary development. The intercept, the linear slope, and the quadratic slope as the three latent growth factors were free to estimate. To determine the number of classes, unconditional models without predictors and covariates with one to five classes were first estimated. The same process was repeated to estimate conditional models. Second, I performed multinomial logistic regression to test whether the vocabulary class membership would be predicted by family literacy environment, classroom process quality, and children’s learning behaviors while controlling for child gender, poverty, mother education, mother marital status, and child ethnicity.