Neglecting to protect: DCS lesions disrupt food protection behavior organization. 1 Blankenship, P.A., 1 Wolf, C. N., 2 Cheatwood, J.L., & 1 Wallace, D.G.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The bead maze: Adapting the water maze task to the human manipulatory scale E.L. Loda, A.B. Thoennes, J.R. Köppen, S.S. Winter, D.A. Hamilton, D.G. Wallace.
Advertisements

Cerebral Glucose Metabolism in Obsessive-Compulsive Hoarding
192-IgG Saporin lesions of the medial septum or nucleus basalis magnocellularis disrupt exploratory trip organization. D.G. Wallace*, S.S. Winter, M.M.
All slides © S. J. Luck, except as indicated in the notes sections of individual slides Slides may be used for nonprofit educational purposes if this copyright.
Cortical Dynamics Underlying Waves of Perceptual Dominance.
Cognitive deficits in methamphetamine addiction Ronald E. See, Ph.D. Department of Neurosciences Medical University of South Carolina Charleston, SC 2011.
Attentionally Dependent Bilateral Advantage on Numerosity Judgments Jenny Ewing & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Neural activity in rodent perirhinal and medial agranular cortices in a Go/Delay-Go task Clayton Aldern—Supervisors: Rebecca Burwell 1 2, Jonathan Ho 1.
Pre-frontal cortex and Executive Function Squire et al Ch 52.
Visual Attention: Outline Levels of analysis 1.Subjective: perception of unattended things 2.Functional: tasks to study components of attention 3.Neurological:
AMPHETAMINE AND HALOPERIDOL MAY ALTER TEMPORAL PROCESSING DURING A NATURALLY OCCURRING BEHAVIOR P.S.Wallace 1* ; E.F.Field 2 ; I.Q.Whishaw 2 ; D.G.Wallace.
Estradiol Exacerbates Anxiety During Acute Methamphetamine Withdrawal in Female Mice Anthony S. Rauhut, Munin Streitz, Alicia Trappanese, Charlotte Woody.
Spatial Neglect and Attention Networks Week 11 Group 1 Amanda Ayoub Alyona Koneva Kindra Akridge Barbara Kim.
Spatial Neglect and Attention Networks
Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S.
Gain Modulation Huei-Ju Chen Papers: Chance, Abbott, and Reyes(2002) E. Salinas & T. Sejnowski(2001) E. Salinas & L.G. Abbott (1997, 1996) Pouget & T.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation disrupts exploratory trip organization D.G. Wallace*; S.K. Knapp; J.A. Silver;
Methods Results Conclusions Walking the plank: Role of the medial septum in distance estimation S.S. Winter; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology,
Humans were able to accurately use dead-reckoning to estimate distance and direction on a smaller scale. Participants modulated their peak speed on the.
A Human Analogue of Rat Food Hoarding Behavior: Searching Path Kinematics Influence the Accuracy of Dead Reckoning S.N. Choudhry; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace*
Conclusions Both groups set up a home base under novel-dark and light conditions and display a similar topographical organization of exploratory trips.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
Methods Results Conclusions Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt self-movement cue processing in a food hoarding task. *S.S. Winter; J.L. McMillin, D.G.
Methods Results Conclusions Getting lost in the dark: Disruption of directional but not distance estimates following mammillothalamic tract lesions *S.S.
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Figure 4: Average dodger speeds are shown for MS (left panel) and NB (right panel) groups. Dodging (circles) and bracing (triangles) speeds are plotted.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Running on rungs: Serial pattern learning on the horizontal ladder *M.J Hylin 1 ; S.S. Winter 1 ; T. L. Marsiglio 1 ;
Participants: 21 smokers (13M, ages 18-45) and 21 age-, gender-, race-, and education-matched controls. Procedure: Stimuli were 100 photographs: 50 food.
Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging.
Brittany N. Dugger 1, John A. Morris 1, Cynthia L. Jordan 1,2, S. Marc Breedlove 1,2 1 Neuroscience Program; 2 Department of Psychology Michigan State.
Animal Models of Episodic Memory? Charan Ranganath Center for Neuroscience and Dept of Psychology, UC Davis Visit us on the web at:
Double Dissociation of Amygdala and Hippocampal Contributions to Trace and Delay Fear Conditioning Jonathan D. Raybuck*, K. Matthew Lattal Department of.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology,
1 Chapter 6 Diencephalon Chris Rorden University of South Carolina Arnold School of Public Health Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders University.
Do Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Ever Return Home? S.L.Bennett 1* ; D.A.Hamilton 2 ; D.G.Wallace 1 1. Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb,
The ‘when’ pathway of the right parietal lobe L. Battelli A. Pascual - LeoneP. Cavanagh.
Zicong Zhang Authors Wendy A. Suzuki Professor of Neural Science and Psychology, New York University Research interest: Organization of memory.
Competitive Place Task
Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes.
Sensorimotor systems Chapters 8.
Background The physiology of the cerebral cortex is organized in hierarchical manner. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) constitutes the highest level of the.
Cortical Stimulation Improves Skilled Forelimb Use Following a Focal Ischemic Infarct in the Rat Campbell Teskey et al, 2003.
Lesions of the Basal Forebrain Cholinergic System Impair Task Acquisition and Abolish Cortical Plasticity Associated with Motor Skill Learning James M.
Studying Memory Encoding with fMRI Event-related vs. Blocked Designs Aneta Kielar.
The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation Group 4 Youngjin Kang Zhiheng Zhou Baoyu Wang.
Determinants of Olfactory Memory Span in Rats MacQueen, D. A., Bullard, L. A., & Galizio, M. University of North Carolina Wilmington Introduction Dudchenko,
The Role of the Basal Ganglia in Habit Formation By Henry H. Yin & Barbara J. Knowlton Group 3, Week 10 Alicia Iafonaro Kimberly Villalva Tawni Voyles.
Raymond P. Kesner, Bridget L. Bolland, Manoli Dakis
A new neural framework for visuospatial processing Group #4 Alicia Iafonaro Alyona Koneva Barbara Kim Isaac Del Rio.
Introduction  The Temporoammonic (TA) projection from entorhinal cortex (EC) layer III to the s. l-m. layer of the CA1 subregion is the direct source.
Memory 2 PSB The Temporal Lobes and Declarative Memory The Diencephalon and Memory Processing –Diencephalon: Brain regions associated with memory.
Dashed line delineates the category boundary Cat A Cat B Visual WithinAcrossAuditory Within Cat A Cat B Cat C Cat D Cat A Cat B Cat C Cat D Dashed lines.
The Differential Engagement of Executive Functions In Word and Contextual Reading After Reading Training In Children With Dyslexia: An fMRI Study Introduction.
Melanie Boysen & Gwendolyn Walton
Timing of SCH23390 Administration Influences Extinction of Conditioned Hyperactivity in Mice Anthony S. Rauhut 1,2, Kristen Ratner2, Sandy Buck2, and Ee-Rah.
Adolescent Behavior and Dopamine Availability Are Uniquely Sensitive to Dietary Omega-3 Fatty Acid Deficiency  Corina O. Bondi, Ameer Y. Taha, Jody L.
Making sense of the senseless: Evaluating sex differences of ASO
NATURE NEUROSCIENCE 2007 Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep Daoyun Ji & Matthew A Wilson Department of Brain.
Effects of inhibition training on event-related potential markers of inhibitory control in heavy drinkers Mrunal Bandawar , MBBS1,2 ; Matt Field, PhD1.
Methods Objectives Results Conclusions References
Volume 55, Issue 3, Pages (August 2007)
Jung Hoon Sul, Hoseok Kim, Namjung Huh, Daeyeol Lee, Min Whan Jung 
Volume 75, Issue 4, Pages (August 2012)
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Adaptive Training Diminishes Distractibility in Aging across Species
Volume 38, Issue 5, Pages (June 2003)
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Brain ischemic tolerance model using mouse MCAO
César F. Lima, Saloni Krishnan, Sophie K. Scott 
Presentation transcript:

Neglecting to protect: DCS lesions disrupt food protection behavior organization. 1 Blankenship, P.A., 1 Wolf, C. N., 2 Cheatwood, J.L., & 1 Wallace, D.G. 1 Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL, USA 2 Department of Anatomy, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, IL USA * C Figure 2:Coronal sections stained with DAPI, a nuclear marker (A); NeuN, a neuronal marker (B); and a combined image (C) are provided for a representative DCS animal. Methods Multimodal neglect is a spatial impairment characterized by an inability to detect stimuli or direct movement to one side of the body (Reep et al., 2004). The dorsocentral striatum (DCS) has been shown to be an important component of a corticostriatal network responsible for egocentric processing (Corwin et al., 1986; Reep & Corwin, 1999). The food protection task has been developed that assesses spatial processing in the egocentric reference frame (Whishaw & Tomie, 1988). The current study evaluated the role of the DCS in food protection behavior. Both egocentric and temporal information processing have been shown to contribute to the organization of food protection behavior. Egocentric processing has been shown to influence the ability to detect an incoming conspecific and successfully protect a food item. Temporal processing has been shown to influence the transition food protection behaviors. Eighteen female Long-Evans rats received unilateral infusions of NMDA (n=8) or saline (n=10) into the DCS. Following surgery, rats served as dodgers in the food protection task. During the food protection task, two rats were placed in a circular arena, the dodger rat was given a food item and a conspecific (robber) attempted to steal the food item. In order to protect the food item, the dodger engaged in two distinct behaviors: 1) a dodge, or large lateral movement away from the robber in which the food item is moved from the forelimbs to the mouth, or 2) a brace, a smaller lateral movement in which the food item remains in the forelimbs and the animal pivots about their hind limbs. Successful protection of the food item depends on accurate egocentric processing of the approaching robber; whereas the propensity of the dodger to elicit one behavior over another has been posited to depend on the estimated time to consume the food item (Whishaw & Gorny, 1994; Wallace et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2008). The current study uses the food protection task to investigate the effects of unilateral DCS lesions on the organization of food protection behavior. Introduction References Figure 1: A photograph of the apparatus used in the current study. Figure 8: Total thefts are plotted by side for control and DCS rats (A). The average distance between noses of dodger and robber at the start of the food protection behavior is plotted for both group relative side of approach (B) and plotted individually for dodges (C) and braces (D). Figure 4: The graphs below represent each group’s average peak speed during open field (A) and peak speeds associated with each type of food protection behavior (B). Histology Apparatus Food Protection Behaviors Results Corwin, J.V., Kanter, S., Watson, R.T., Heilman, K.M., Valenstein, E., & Hashimoto, A. (1986). Apomorphine has a therapeutic effect on neglect produced by unilateral dorsomedial prefrontal cortex lesions in rats. Experimental Neurology, 94: Martin, M.M., Winter, S.S., Cheatwood, J.L., Carter, L.A., Jones, J.L., Weathered, S.L., Wagner, S.J., Wallace, D.G., (2008). Organization of food protection behavior is differentially influenced by 192 IgG- saporin lesions of either the medial septum or the nucleus basalis magnocellularis. Brain Research. 1241, Reep, R.L., & Corwin, J.V. (1999). Topographic organization of the striatal and thalamic connections of rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Research, Reep, R.L., Corwin, J.V., Cheatwood, J.L., Van Vleet, T.M., Heilman, K.M., Watson, R.T,(2004). A rodent model for investigating the neurobiology of contralateral neglect. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 17 (4), Wallace, D.G., Wallace, P.S., Field, E., Whishaw, I.Q., (2006). Pharmacological manipulations of food protection behavior in rats: evidence for dopaminergic contributions to time perception during a natural behavior. Brain Research (1), Whishaw, I.Q., Tomie, J.A., (1988). Food wrenching and dodging: a neuro-ethological test of cortical and dopaminergic contributions to sensorimotor behavior in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 102 (1), Whishaw, I.Q., Gorny, B.P., (1994). Food wrenching and dodging: eating time estimates influence dodge probability and amplitude. Aggressive Behavior. 20, Unilateral DCS lesions did not result in motivational deficits. No group differences were observed in the total distance traveled during Open Field No group differences were observed in the time it took to consume the food item. Groups exhibited similar amounts of behaviors exhibited during food protection sessions Unilateral DCS lesions influenced temporal factors related to food protection behavior. DCS rats transitioned between food protection behaviors later than control animals Unilateral DCS lesions influenced sensorimotor factors related to food protection behavior. DCS rats exhibited lateralized impairments in their ability to protect the food item from theft. The food protection task may provide an novel tool to dissociate neural systems that mediate temporal and spatial processing. Figure 6: The graphs shown below represent total dodges (A) and braces (B) by sample for the control and DCS animals. DCS animals exhibited significantly fewer braces during samples 1-4. A A B A A B D A B C C B Figure 5: The graphs below represent the total distance traveled during open field(A), the average time it took to consume a food pellet (B),each group’s total number of behaviors (C), and the average number of thefts for each group (D). No significant group differences were observed in these measures. A B C Figure 3: The figures below represent the topographic and kinematic characteristics of typical dodging (A,B) and bracing (C,D) behaviors. Conclusions * Figure 7: The graphs represent the behavioral transition by sample for sham (A) and DCS (B) animals. The average sample in which behavioral transitions occurred is plotted for both groups (C). DCS group transitioned significantly later than the sham group. D B * ** * A B CD