(Results cont.) Q3. What is the level of agreement for using deception in a social interaction setting, or therapist interaction setting? H3. People will.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship Desirability Within Hook-up Culture: Nice-Guys vs. Bad-Boys An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship.
Advertisements

Conflict, Supportive Communication, and Group Satisfaction Katlynn Balson, Laura Turner, Virgil Ward II, Alexandra Zaic Faculty Mentor: Dr. Martha Fay.
Method Introduction Results Discussion The Effect of Self-Esteem, Marital Status, and Gender on Trait Anxiety and Stress Emily B Gale University of Nebraska-Lincoln.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © s.com Spirituality and Experiential Avoidance in Social Anxiety Benjamin Ramos, Elizabeth Mejia-Muñoz, Michael.
Social Anxiety and Depression Comorbidity Influences on Positive Alcohol Expectancies Amy K. Bacon, Hilary G. Casner, & Lindsay S. Ham University of Arkansas.
Cultural Differences in Approaches to Arbitration Peter B. Smith University of Sussex Brunel University May 24, 2013.
Students’ Perceptions of the Physiques of Self and Physical Educators
Introduction ► College-student drinking remains a significant problem on campuses across the nation. ► It is estimated that 38-44% of college students.
Individual Preferences for Uncertainty: An Ironically Pleasurable Stimulus Bankert, M., VanNess, K., Hord, E., Pena, S., Keith, V., Urecki, C., & Buchholz,
Problem-Solving Abilities and Feelings of Control: A Work in Progress Emily M. Kaiser, Department of Communication Studies, College of Arts and Sciences.
Acceptability of Lying as Function of Perceivers’ Motive to Lie, Relative Importance of the Situation, and Closeness to the Person by Faiza Moin, Anila.
Attractive Equals Smart? Perceived Intelligence as a Function of Attractiveness and Gender Abstract Method Procedure Discussion Participants were 38 men.
Some factors leading to initial attraction Proximity (more likely to form relationships with those who live near us, or that we interact with on a regular.
Personally Important Posttraumatic Growth as a Predictor of Self-Esteem in Adolescents Leah McDiarmid, Kanako Taku Ph.D., & Aundreah Walenski Presented.
Template provided by: “posters4research.com”   Ideals: mental constructs that represent an idea of traits we are attracted to in potential partners (Fletcher.
Background There is a long literature documenting greater willingness to take risks by men than by women. This gender difference in risk taking has been.
Homelessness and Mental Illness: The Medical Students’ Viewpoints Charity Pires BS, Sarah Hilton MS, Faneece Embry BS, Anthony Ahmed PhD, Edna Stirewalt.
Mankind’s Greatest Blessing: Personal Humor Style and Humor Appreciation By: Steven LaCorte The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between.
Childhood Trauma as a Mediator of the Relationship between Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Recognition Julie A. Meinert Borderline Personality.
Wendy L. Wolfe, Kaitlyn Patterson, & Hannah Towhey
Kaitlyn Patterson & Wendy Wolfe
INTERPERSONAL Attraction
Better to Give or to Receive?: The Role of Dispositional Gratitude
Reversing the Error: The Role of Causal Attributions in
Paul B. Ingram IV2 and Brian P. Cole1
The Relationship Between Instagram Photo Editing and Undergraduate College Women’s Body Dissatisfaction Madeline Wick, Cindy Miller-Perrin, & Jennifer.
Optimism is Weakly and Not Significantly Related to Decision Making
Motivated to lie: The effects of incentives on characteristics of truthful and deceptive informant reports Carroll A. Boydell & J. Don Read Figure 1 Figure.
Murat KEZER1 Barış SEVİ1, Zeynep CEMALCILAR1, & Lemi BARUH2
Chapter 4 Research Methods in Clinical Psychology
Sexual Imagery & Thinking About Sex
DECEPTION ACROSS DIFFERENT MODES OF COMMUNICATION
University of Mount Union
ERFCON th International Conference of the Faculty of Education and Rehabilitation Sciences University of Zagreb 17 – 19 May 2017, Zagreb SOCIAL COMPETENCE.
The Disclosure of Virginity Status and Sexual Orientation
Christian Hahn, M.Sc. & Lorne Campbell, PhD
Nutrition Education Intervention
Multiculturalism and Its Links to Hostility
Introduction Hypotheses Results Discussion Method
SAINT LOUIS UNIVERSITY
Once again… WHAT DO YOU SEE?
Religiosity and Romantic Beliefs
Evaluating the Distinction Between Aversive Indecisiveness and Procrastination: Relationships with Anxiety, Anxiety Vulnerability, and Personality Traits.
Friendship Quality as a Moderator
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Participants and Procedures
Which of these is “a boy”?
Research amongst Physical Therapists in the State of Kuwait: Participation, Perception, Attitude and Barriers Presented by Sameera Aljadi, PT, PhD Assistant.
My, But We are Impressive
Sociosexuality and Perceptions of Partner Over Time
The Influence of Psychopathy on Third Party Assignment of Blame
Predicting Variations in Motivations for Romantic Kissing
Perceived versus Actual Knowledge of Autism Spectrum Disorder
Two randomised controlled crossover studies to evaluate the effect of colouring on both self-report and performance measures of well-being Holt, N. J.,
Thesis Proposal Presentation
Introduction Measures
Charles N. Elliott, Paul A. Story
Effects of Mood on Children’s Stigmatization of Overweight Peers
Emily A. Davis & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Prosocial Behaviors in Adolescence
General Social Competence (18)
Korey F. Beckwith & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Aashna A. Dhayagude & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Demographic Characteristics of the Sample DISCUSSION/CASE STUDY
Abstract The purpose of this study was to validate the association between multiple stressful life occurrences and posttraumatic growth (PTG) in an American.
Serik Tursunaliev, Veronica Ross
Jocks, Mathletes, and Band Geeks; Do Extracurricular Activities Affect Perceptions of Future and Rule Violating Behaviors? Kelly Jacques and Sarah Nordstrom.
The Effect of Lineup Structure on Individual Identification
CHINWAG Emma Bergman, Christian Gutierrez, Peter Louden, Cassie Qiu
Daniela Mercado, Sophie Barbagallo, & Olivia Downey
Presentation transcript:

(Results cont.) Q3. What is the level of agreement for using deception in a social interaction setting, or therapist interaction setting? H3. People will agree with the use of deception in a social setting. More specifically, it is predicted that the level of agreement for using deception in a social interaction will be greater for self-oriented lies compared to other- or both-oriented lies. As predicted, there was a statistically significant difference between groups, F (5, 97) = 2.70, p =.025, ɳ p 2 =.80, indicating people agreed more with the use of deception in a social setting compared to lying in the therapy interaction setting. There was a statistically significant main effect of the orientation of benefit (self, other, and relationship), F (2,100) = 3.67, p <.029, ɳ p 2 =.07. While not expected, there was a significant difference in orientation of benefit based on type of deception as shown in Table 4 indicating people had a greater agreement for certain types of deception based on the benefit of orientation. Q4. What are the relational effects of using deception in a social interaction, or therapist interaction? H4. People will view deception as being destructive compared to being helpful to the relationship in both settings. Specifically, the use of deception will be viewed as being destructive to the relationship compared to being destructive to themselves, or the other person. As such, deception will be viewed as being more destructive to the relationship in the therapy interaction setting compared to the social interaction setting. Contrary to the prediction that the level of destructiveness would be greater for the relationships, results indicated there was a significant main effect of the level of destructiveness rated, such that the mean ratings of destructiveness were significantly different for self (M = 3.38, SD =.56) and others (M = 3.26, SD =.56), p =.001, but not significantly different between self (M = 3.38, SD =.56) and relationship (M = 3.35, SD =.61), p =.433. Mean ratings of destructiveness were also significantly different between relationship (M = 3.35, SD =.61) and others (M = 3.26, SD =.56), p =.005. Opposing to the hypothesis, there was not a significant difference in level of destructiveness based on groups, F(5,94) = 1.13, p =.326, ɳ p 2 =.024 indicating deception was not perceived to be more destructive to one setting compared to the other. Q5. Would people prefer to use deception versus having a conflict with another person in the social interaction setting, or therapy interaction setting? H5. People will endorse the use of deception for both settings in order to avoid conflict. As predicted, there was not a significant difference between groups indicating deception was preferred in both settings as a means to avoid conflict, F (5, 99) = 1.16, p =.33, ɳ p 2 =.06. Results Q1. Does the social situational setting of an interaction motivate the frequency and likelihood of deception to occur? H1. The participant’s reported frequency and likelihood to use deception in the social setting will be greater than the occurrence of deception in the therapy condition. There was a statistically significant main effect of the type of lie rated, F (5, 99) = 11.87, p <.001, ɳ p 2 =.38 in which there was a greater frequency to use certain some types of lies, and others lest often. Contrary to predictions, there was not a significant difference between groups, F (5, 99) = 1.33, p =.26, ɳ p 2 =.06 indicating there was not a greater frequency to use deception in the social setting compared to the therapy setting. A mixed ANOVA compared the likelihood to use deception with types of lies as the repeated measures variable and setting as the between measures variable. Also contrary to the hypothesis, there was not a significant difference between groups, F (5, 101) = 1.49, p =.20, ɳ p 2 =.07 indicating there was not a greater likelihood to use deception in the social setting compared to the therapy setting. Q2. What is the level of acceptability of using deception in a social interaction? H2A. People will hold beliefs that lying in social situations is more acceptable than unacceptable. In addition, it is expected that there will be a correlation between acceptability of lying in social situations with a global acceptability of lying, indicated by the Revised Lie Acceptability Scale. Consistent with the hypothesis, lie acceptability scores for the therapy condition on the MDQ were significantly lower by a mean of 3.08, SD = 1.35, 95% CI [-1.14 to -0.71], than a neutral lie acceptability score of 4.0, t(50) = -8.75, p <.001. However, contrary to the prediction, lie acceptability scores for the social condition on the MDQ were also significantly lower by a mean of 3.44, SD = 1.33, 95% CI [-0.78 to -0.35], than a neutral lie acceptability score of 4.0, t(53) = -5.35, p <.001. Additionally, one-sample t-tests were conducted on each group’s average RLAS score to determine if they were significantly different from an anchor of 32 (mid-point), which would indicate a statistically significant difference in lie acceptability (Bonferroni correction =.025). Consistent with the prediction, lie acceptability scores for the therapy condition on the RLAS were significantly lower by a mean of 23.35, SD = 8.12, 95% CI [ to -6.40], than a mid-point lie acceptability score of 32, t(51) = -7.70, p <.001. However, lie acceptability scores for the social condition on the RLAS were significantly lower by a mean of 22.71, SD = 8.16, 95% CI [ to -7.08], than a mid-point lie acceptability score of 32, t(54) = -8.44, p <.001. Consistent with predictions, there was a significant correlation found between the RLAS and total lie acceptability score, r = -0.20, n= 105, p = 0.04, indicating a relationship between the two variables. H2B. People will view lying in social interactions more acceptable compared to the therapy interaction settings. In support of the hypothesis, there was a significant difference between groups, such that people in the social condition (M = 3.44, SD =.78) gave deception a better acceptability rating compared to those in the Therapy group (M = 3.08, SD =.76 ), F (5, 99) = 5.80, p =.018, ɳ p2 =.05. However, deception in both settings was perceived to be unacceptable. Pairwise comparisons within groups revealed several significant comparisons as shown in Figure 2. Higher ratings indicate greater acceptability. Figure 1 Discussion This research contributes to our understanding motivations for deception in therapy. Overall, the majority of findings indicated that the use of deception was endorsed in both settings, and that therapy and social situations are not distinctly different in eliciting deception. These findings further support that deception is a part of everyday life, and the notion that white lies are the most common type of deception used. Results from the current study indicate that overall, the endorsement of deception in both the therapy and social setting is correlated with a global acceptability of lying indicated by the Revised Lie Acceptability Scale. As such, the use of the RLAS may be a helpful in determining the endorsement of using deception. While deception was perceived to be unacceptable in both settings, white lies received the greatest rating of acceptability which is consistent with previous research. Results of this study support that white lies are commonly and socially acceptable because they are perceived to be altruistic in nature. Overall, deception was viewed as being more destructive than helpful in both settings of interaction. Results indicate that people found the overall use of deception to be low on acceptability. In addition, deception was also perceived as destructive. These findings may indicate that because deception is destructive in nature, it is therefore perceived to be unacceptable to use. Further, as predicted, people endorsed the use of deception for both settings in order to avoid conflict. The findings of the current study may be based on the perception that the cost of telling the truth would be more damaging to the parties involved in the interactions than the use of deceptive strategies. Therefore, deception may have been perceived as being the better option between the two choices. Results from this study have important implications for research that evaluates situational influences that motivate the use of deception. In particular, results imply that a social situation and professional helping situation do not elicit different levels of deception. Therapists and other professionals in the counseling and psychotherapy fields can benefit from this study by understanding that the occurrence of deception in therapy may not be much different than in social settings, regarding its frequency and use. Overall, this study further contributes to the current deception literature and to the practice of counseling and clinical psychology by seeking to expand our understanding of deception in the therapeutic setting. Introduction The simple truth is, lying is a part of everyday life. Results of several studies have indicated that people lie on average, about two times a day (DePaulo & Bell, 1996; DePaulo & Kashy, 1998; Kashy & DePaulo, 1996, Vrij, 2000, Sosa, et. al., 2014). While everyone lies, people differ in their motivations for using deception. Vrij (2008) provides reasons for why people lie based on the findings of several studies. He reported that the occurrence of deception can depend on the type of relationship involved. This is supported by DePaulo and Kashy’s (1998) study of deception and the emotional closeness of a relationship. Their findings indicated that the highest frequencies of lying occurred in conversations with strangers and the lowest rate occurred in conversations with spouses. Knowing that deception occurs on a daily basis, it is important to understand the nuances of deception. Many people have different thoughts and understandings of what constitutes deception and what does not. Therefore, it is important to evaluate the many shades of deception and what motivates deception to occur. Little attention has been given to the setting in which detection occurs. As such, literature in this area of deception can reap benefits for forensic contexts, social relationships, dating relationships, workplace interviews and relationships, or even everyday interactions. Method Participants There were 84 women (77.8%) and 24 men (22.2%) raging in age from 18 to 38 years (M = 19.78, SD = 2.80) with self-reported race as African American/Black (7.4%), Caucasian/European American (50.9%), Asian/Asian American/Pacific Islander (1.9%), Hispanic/Latina/Latino (32.4%), Bi-Racial (4.6%) and Multi-racial (2.8%). Procedures Participants were randomly assigned to either the social interaction condition, or the therapy condition (Figure 1). Thus, participants in the therapy condition were asked to view the six types of deception in a therapy context and participants in the social condition were asked to view the 6 types of deception in a social/casual setting. However, for both conditions the sequence of videos mirrored the order of the vignettes used in Peterson’s (1996) study: 1) Omission scenario, 2) Half-truth scenario, 3) White Lie scenario, 4) Exaggeration scenario, 5) Blatant Lie scenario, 6) Failed Lie scenario. Participants were instructed to view the six types of deception videos, and fill out the Motivations for Deception Questionnaire upon the completion of each video. Following the videos and Motivations for Deception Questionnaire, participants were prompted to complete the Revised Lie Acceptability Scale and Demographics Questionnaire. Therapy Setting Social Setting References DePaulo, B. M., & Bell, K. L. (1996). Truth and investment: lies are told to those who care. Journal of personality and social psychology, 71(4), 703. DePaulo, B. M., & Kashy, D. A. (1998). Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, doi: / Kashy, D. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1996). Who lies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), doi: / Sosa, S. L., Gonzales, H. C., Curtis, D.A., & Hart, C. L. (2014, May). Deception in therapy: frequencies, types, and therapist relationship. Poster presented at the 26th APS Annual Convention, San Francisco, CA. Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit: The psychology of lying and the implications for professional practice. West, Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities (2 nd Ed.). West, Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Type of LieMean (SD)Types of LieMean (SD)Mean Diff.Sig. Omission2.80 (1.30)Half Truth3.22 (1.39) * White Lie3.82 (1.50) * Exaggeration3.76 (1.24) * Blatant Lie2.90 (1.36) Failed Dec.3.06 (1.39) Half Truth3.22 (1.39)White Lie3.82 (1.50) * Exaggeration3.76 (1.24) * Blatant Lie2.90 (1.36) * Failed Dec.3.06 (1.39) White Lie3.82 (1.50)Exaggeration3.76 (1.24) Blatant Lie2.90 (1.36) * Failed Dec.3.06 (1.39) * Exaggeration3.76 (1.24)Blatant Lie2.90 (1.36) * Failed Dec.3.06 (1.39) * Blatant Lie Failed Dec.3.06 (1.39) Figure 2. Acceptability: Type of Lie Pairwise Comparisons Within Groups *. The mean difference is significant at the.05 level. Faculty Advisors: Drew A. Curtis, Ph.D Tay Hack, Ph.D. Jungeun Lee, Ph.D..