What makes effective Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimuli? Classical conditioning procedures do not always result in learning Just pairing two stimuli.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Facebook Group: The group is called: Psych281 Spring08 Available only to University of Alberta network Sorry to be rude but… Please don’t add me as a friend.
Advertisements

Chapter 4 – Classical Conditioning: Mechanisms Important characteristics of the CS and US –1) Novelty of CS and US Latent Inhibition –association account.
Theories of Classical Conditioning
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning: Part 2 Lesson 10.
Introduction to Psychology, 7th Edition, Rod Plotnik Module 9: Classical Conditioning Module 9 Classical Conditioning.
Conditioned Inhibition
Introduction to Learning and Behavior, 3e by Russell A. Powell, Diane G. Symbaluk, and P. Lynne Honey Copyright © 2009 Wadsworth Publishing, a division.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. 3 Kinds of Learning l Classical Conditioning n Kind of learning in which a neutral stimulus acquires the ability to produce.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning MR. McKinley First a quick video… games/videos/pavlovs-bell.htm
Lectures 7&8: Pavlovian Conditioning (Determining Conditions) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education Canada Inc. Learning Chapter 5.
Classical Conditioning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Psychology 2250 Last class Definition of learning
Learning Theories Goal  How do we learn behaviors through classical conditioning?
Chapter 3 - Principles & Applications
4 th Edition Copyright 2004 Prentice Hall5-1 Learning Chapter 5.
Learning Ms. Simon Do Now: Define Learning. Definition Learning is a relatively permanent change in an organism’s behavior due to experience.
Psychology 2250 Last Class Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization -time course -stimulus-specificity -effects of strong extraneous stimuli (dishabituation)
Innate Behavior Patterns Reflex Tropism –kinesis (undirected) –taxis (directed) Fixed Action Pattern –species-specific; unlearned; goes to completion Reaction.
Conditioning / I. Learning / A. Any relatively permanent change in behavior as a result of practice or experience. Changes due to growth or maturation.
Lecture 2: Classical Conditioning. Types of learning Habituation and sensitization Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning Instrumental (Operant) conditioning.
Experimental Evidence  Rats drink little saccharin water at first but increase over time.  Loud tones (110 db) produce different responses depending.
LEE JI HOON. question 1. What is the meaning of evolutionary psychology. 2. Why is the taste aversion learning important.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. THREE KINDS OF LEARNING Learning –A relatively enduring or permanent change in behavior that results from previous experience.
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. 3 Kinds of Learning l Classical Conditioning n Kind of learning in which a neutral stimulus acquires the ability to produce.
Learning. Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning Changing Behavior Through Reinforcement and Punishment: Operant Conditioning Learning by Insight.
Nonassociative Learning
An Introduction to THEORIES of LEARNING CHAPTER An Introduction to Theories of Learning, Ninth Edition Matthew H. Olson | B. R. Hergenhahn Copyright ©
Learning & Memory JEOPARDY. The Field CC Basics Important Variables Theories Grab Bag $100 $200$200 $300 $500 $400 $300 $400 $300 $400 $500 $400.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
ATTRIBUTES OF LEARNING AND CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. Objectives Students will be able to… Students will be able to… Discuss the stages of Classical Conditioning Discuss the.
Learning Long lasting, relatively permanent change in behavior due to experience.
Simple and primitive form of learning involving only glandular and visceral responses like salivation CS-US associations occur automatically through the.
Rescorla-Wagner Model  US-processing model  Can account for some Pavlovian Conditioning phenomena: acquisition blocking unblocking with an upshift conditioned.
Stimulus Control of Behavior
Concepts of Habituation and Sensitization
The Rescorla-Wagner Model
Introduction to Approaches in Psychology Learning & Conditioning
AP Psychology Unit: Learning.
Classical Conditioning
Long lasting change in behavior due to experience.
Unit 4: Memory & Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Unit: Learning.
Classical Conditioning: Learning by Response
Basic Learning Concepts and Classical Conditioning
LEARNING Chapter 6.
The Role of Biology in Conditioning
Classical Conditioning
Attributes of Learning and Classical Conditioning
Classical Conditioning and prediction
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning
Aim: How does classical conditioning impact behavior?
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Demonstration of Learning
The Role of Biology in Conditioning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning
Classical Conditioning
Learning.
Classical and Operant Conditioning
Introduction to Approaches in Psychology Learning & Conditioning
Agenda To Get: To Do: Guided notes Intro Unit 7: Learning
Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior that occurs as a result of experience. Associative Learning- learning to associate 2 events,
Presentation transcript:

What makes effective Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimuli? Classical conditioning procedures do not always result in learning Just pairing two stimuli together does not necessarily result in information being stored in the brain, depends on: – timing of stimuli – characteristics of the CS – characteristics of the US

Initial responses to the stimuli CS – does not elicit the CR initially (usually biologically weak) US – elicits the target response without any special training (usually biologically strong) Identification of CS and US can be relative – A particular event may serve as a CS relative to one stimulus and serve as a US relative to another stimulus – Sucrose food pellets – a tasty treat that rats like Sucrose food pellets can serve as the CS in a taste-aversion experiment where illness is the US The same sucrose food pellets could serve as the US in sign tracking – light ---> Saccharin pairings in a sign tracking procedure (in this case sweet taste is a proxy for food) – The animal would approach the light – Food is a special case because it has sensory cues, visual, odor, taste as well as biological consequences, nutrition, toxins and so on – Some stimuli are biologically weak Quiet sounds, dim light, constant touch

The Novelty of Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimuli The behavioral impact of a stimulus depends on its novelty Familiar stimuli do not elicit as vigorous a response as do novel stimuli (i.e., Habituation) Stimulus novelty is also important for classical conditioning If the CS and/or the US are familiar, conditioning proceeds more slowly than if they were novel (note: can still get conditioning) Testing the role of stimulus novelty involves 2 phases: – (1) preexposure of the stimulus only – (2) conditioning of CS – US

CS-Preexposure effect or Latent Inhibition Phase 1: Preexposure - Subjects are given repeated presentations of the CS alone – is similar to habituation because the repeated preexposure to the CS limits the processing of, or attention to the stimulus – is due to a stimulus-specific decline of attention – more processing of new inputs and less processing of older unimportant ones – helps preserve limited attentional resources Phase 2: Conditioning - Subjects are given pairings of the CS with the US – Preexposore of the CS retards learning Recent models of latent inhibition are based on retrieval competition concepts

Evidence That latent inhibition Is Modulated by Attentional Processes modulation of latent inhibition by attentional processes that are governed by attentional variables latent inhibition is reduced by manipulations that maintain or restore attention – a second, neutral, stimulus is paired with the target stimulus on each of its preexposure presentations (masking task) – salience of the stimulus CS-preexposure effect or Latent Inhibition

produce behavioral effects that are purportedly related to schizophrenia. behavioral tests have been latent inhibition, prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response, or social interaction latent inhibition measures for Schizophrenia based on the assumption that latent inhibition effects in animals and humans are governed by the same attentional processes selective attention disorder, as represented by high distractibility, is central to schizophrenia nonmedicated acute schizophrenia patients show reduced latent inhibition compared to normal subjects latent inhibition can be demonstrated with reaction time (RT) to a target stimulus (the US) which is predicted by a cue (the CS). healthy subjects show latent inhibition during the early stages of the formation of the cue–target associations and that latent inhibition would be attenuated with further cue–target pairings, chronic schizophrenia less have latent inhibition initially but have perseveration of latent inhibition Latent Inhibition Models of Schizophrenia

Nature of Attentional Dysfunction in Schizophrenia the amount of attention allocated to a stimulus is equivalent to the amount or depth of processing normal latent inhibition depends on a shift in processing mode from controlled to automatic The disrupted latent inhibition in schizophrenia patients is due to a failure to shift from controlled to automatic processing Latent Inhibition Models of Schizophrenia

US-preexposure effect Experiments on the importance of US novelty are designed in a similar way to CS novelty Phase 1: Preexposure - Subjects are given repeated presentations of the US alone Phase 2: Conditioning - Subjects are given pairings of the CS with the US The initial preexposure to the US usually retards subsequent Conditioning This may be due to conditioning of background cues during the preexposure phase Pre-exposure effects have two explanations – Associative Interference - stimuli are less able to form associations – Memory Interference - memory of preexposure interferes with memory of conditioning

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

CS and US Intensity and Salience Stimulus salience – refers to the significance or noticeability of the stimulus – Within limits Learning occurs more rapidly with more salient, or more noticeable stimuli – Intensity increases salience due to greater noticeability – Stimuli relevant to the biological needs of the animal (i.e., can increase the salience of a salt taste by depriving the animal of salt) CS Salience – A more intense CS also results in better conditioning This makes sense from an ecological point of view a more intense CS will be more salient than the background and will command more attention With CTA, a stronger concentration of flavor results in bigger taste aversions – using a CS similar to stimuli animals might encounter in the natural environment such as realistic quail models, see figure 4.2

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

CS-US Relevance, or Belongingness Rate of Classical Conditioning is also affected by the extent to which the CS is relevant to or belongs with the US – Some CS-US combinations are better than others i.e. – Genetic predisposition for selective associations between certain combinations of CSs and USs – The importance of stimulus relevance was demonstrated in an experiment by Garcia & Koelling (1966) see figure 4.3 and figure 4.4 They compared learning about peripheral pain (footshock) and learning about nausea type of illness In nature, rats are likely to get sick after eating poisonous food, but experience peripheral pain after an encounter with a predator Rats that got poisoned decreased saccharin consumption Rats that got shocked decreased consumption of the ‘bright and noisy’ water This experiment suggests that tastes are more readily associated with sickness than with shock (peripheral pain) and that audiovisual cues (LN) are more readily associated with pain/shock than with sickness We see evidence of this selectivity of association in other species – For example, certain birds locate food by its visual characteristics, rather than by its taste, rats find food mostly by odor, foxes use auditory information

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Learning without an Unconditioned Stimulus In the early research on classical conditioning the US - UR relationship was seen as essential for learning Some sort of physiological reflex or response system that could be conditioned with a sensory cue the CS This is OK for many basic examples such as salivary reflex, eyeblink or even nausea from eating bad food However there are applied situations that are more complex then this where identifying a US - UR relationship may not be possible Cues in the environment, such as sounds, odors or visual stimuli can be associated with each other. Wolves may detect an odor, hear a sound and see some movement while searching for food before actually getting contact with the food.

Higher-order Conditioning Historical background – CSs often elicit fewer and weaker responses than the US – Pavlov considered the difference in “biological strength” between CSs and USs to be necessary for their effectiveness as CSs and USs – Initially, the CS is weaker than the US – But after conditioning, the CS is strong and elicits a vigorous response – Could the strong CS now serve as a US? Procedure : See figure 4.5 – A stimulus (CS) that has become strongly conditioned may serve as a US – CS1(tone) - US(food); CS1(tone) -> CR (first-order) – CS2(light) - CS1(tone); CS2(light) -> CR (second-order) – CS3(buzzer) - CS2(light); CS3(buzzer) -> CR (third-order) Examples – Money is a powerful CS for humans although money is not always closely paired with a US buy some food and then eat the food later – Crowds can be a powerful fear stimulus (CS) for some people without ever being paired with an aversive US.

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Sensory Preconditioning Both stimuli elicit weak biological responses before conditioning See figure 4.6 – CS2(light) -> CS1(tone) sequential pairing – CS1(tone) -> US(food) -> CR – Test: CS2(light) -> CR – Note: the Domjan book example of vanilla - cinnamon in food is a confusing example because food can be a US and the flavors are presented simultaneously. This challenges the concept of differential biological strength Biological Strength as a facilitator of performance rather than learning In sensory preconditioning, the initial CS2->CS1 association is “mute” does not produce a response Making the CS1 biologically strong, allows expression of the association with CS2 Not all associations are directly evident in behavior

The Principles of Learning and Behavior, 7e by Michael Domjan Copyright © 2015 Wadsworth Publishing, a division of Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.