Psycholınguıstıcs Hakan cangır

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Advertisements

Unit 3 Review 1.Two processes that cause forgetting are decay and displacement. (a) Define each. (b) Both processes are examples of _____ failure.
Cognitive - knowledge.ppt © 2001 Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D.1 Knowledge Structure of semantic memory –relationships among concepts –organization of memory –memory.
Chapter 9 Knowledge.
Chapter 7 Knowledge Terms: concept, categorization, prototype, typicality effect, object concepts, rule-governed, exemplars, hierarchical organization,
Knowledge ß How do we organize our knowledge? ß How do we access our knowledge? ß Do we really use categories?
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Mental representations II.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Representing language.
Knowing Semantic memory.
Natural Categories Hierarchical organization of categories –Superordinate (e.g., furniture) –Basic-level (e.g., chair) –Subordinate (e.g., armchair) Rosch.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Some basic linguistic theory part3.
Knowledge information that is gained and retained what someone has acquired and learned organized in some way into our memory.
Categorization  How do we organize our knowledge?  How do we retrieve knowledge when we need it?
Psycholinguistics 05 Internal Lexicon.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Lexical selection Lexical access How do we retrieve the linguistic information from Long-term memory? What factors are involved.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 8 Semantic Memory.
The Study of Meaning in Language
Lecture 10 – Semantic Networks 1 Two questions about knowledge of the world 1.How are concepts stored? We have already considered prototype and other models.
General Knowledge Dr. Claudia J. Stanny EXP 4507 Memory & Cognition Spring 2009.
Semantic Memory Memory for meaning
Semantic Memory Psychology Introduction This is our memory for facts about the world This is our memory for facts about the world How do we know.
Conceptual Hierarchies Arise from the Dynamics of Learning and Processing: Insights from a Flat Attractor Network Christopher M. O’ConnorKen McRaeGeorge.
Ch. 5 The Internal Lexicon
PSY 323 – COGNITION Chapter 9: Knowledge.  Categorization ◦ Process by which things are placed into groups  Concept ◦ Mental groupings of similar objects,
Semantics The study of meaning in language. Semantics is…  The study of meaning in language.  It deals with the meaning of words (Lexical semantics)
Skilled Reading for New Teachers. Focus Questions What general principles seem to hold true regardless of the subject matter we are teaching? What general.
Category Structure Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 05/20 /2015: Lecture 08-2 This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros.
Concepts and Knowledge Thomas G. Bowers, Ph.D. Penn State Harrisburg 2000.
Long Term Memory: Semantic Kimberley Clow
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 5 – Meaning-Based Knowledge Representation.
1 How is knowledge stored? Human knowledge comes in 2 varieties: Concepts Concepts Relations among concepts Relations among concepts So any theory of how.
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
Organization of Semantic Memory Typical empirical testing paradigm: propositional verification task – rt to car has four wheels vs. car is a status symbol.
Associative Theories of Long- Term Memory. Network Theory The basic notion that we need to explore is that memory consists of a large number of associations.
Semantic Memory Psychology Introduction This is our memory for facts about the world How do we know that the capital of Viet Nam is Hanoi How is.
Verbal Representation of Knowledge
Lecture 8 – Categories 1 great detail is needed for planning & executing current responses some detail about context of new learning can be emcoded with.
VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION. What is Word Recognition? Features, letters & word interactions Interactive Activation Model Lexical and Sublexical Approach.
Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”
Module 5 Other Knowledge Representation Formalisms
Representational Hypotheses in Cognitive Psychology
Semantic Priming Effects in a Bilingual Gujarati Speaker
PeerWise Student Instructions
What is cognitive psychology?
Vocabulary Module 2 Activity 5.
Object-Orientated Programming
Consumer Markets and Consumer Buyer Behavior
Cognitive Processes in SLL and Bilinguals:
Selin Gulgoz Susan A. Gelman University of Michigan Introduction
PSY 323 – Cognition Chapter 9: Knowledge.
Chapter 7 Psychology: Memory.
Theories of Language Development
THE QUESTIONS—SKILLS ANALYSE EVALUATE INFER UNDERSTAND SUMMARISE
Preparing for the Verbal Reasoning Measure
What is Linguistics? The scientific study of human language
THE NATURE of LEARNER LANGUAGE
Class Schedule In-text Citations Long-term Memory: Organization
[Human Memory] 10.Knowledge
Knowledge Pt 2 Chapter 10 Knowledge Pt 2.
Implicit Memory SNU CSE Son, Suil.
Consumer Markets and Consumer Buyer Behavior
Consumer Markets and Consumer Buyer Behavior
The Nature of Learner Language (Chapter 2 Rod Ellis, 1997) Page 15
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
The Study of Meaning in Language
Cognitive Psychology Chapter 7 Knowledge, Part I.
Competence and performance
How is knowledge stored?
The Network Approach: Mind as a Web
Categories My dog sleeping. My dog. All golden retrievers. All dogs. All canines. All mammals… Each of these is a category. Categorization is the process.
Presentation transcript:

Psycholınguıstıcs Hakan cangır Internal Lexicon Psycholınguıstıcs Hakan cangır

Psycholinguistic perspective Representation of words in permanent memory is our internal lexicon Properties Relation to other words meaning spelling pronunciation

Psycholinguistic perspective The process by which we activate these meanings is called lexical access. ACTIVATION Perception of the word Meaning through other words

MEANING What is meaning? Aspects of meaning The relationship between words and things in the world is termed the reference of a word; the things in the world are called the referents of the word Eg. There is a brown cow grazing in the field. We need to grasp its truth conditions to make sense of this sentence. There must be a cow, must be Brown, must be grazing in the field. sense reference

MEANING Not all reference is easy. Eg. Abstract words: justice, plausibility, and relativity. Others are meaningful but have no real referents, such as unicorn. Reference is part of meaning, but there is more to meaning than reference. Two different words or expressions may have the same reference but not mean the same thing. Eg. The prime minister of Turkey – The leader of AKP It is currently the same person. But the meaning of the two expressions are different, as can be seen when a new, different party comes to power.

MEANING Similarly the sentence below is true for now, but may not be after the next election. Eg. The leader of AKP is the prime minister of Turkey The part of meaning that is not its reference is termed its sense. The sense of a Word means its place in a system of relationships which it contracts with other words in the vocabulary. (Lyon, 1968 p. 427)

Relations Synonymy (fear – panic) Antonymy (big – small) Incompatibility (Rebecca wore a red dress vs. Rebecca wore a green dress Hyponymy (dog is a subordinate of animal, a coordinate of cat, and a superordinate of Geman shephard

Sense and reference are complementary aspects of meaning Implication If one expression is true, then another must be true. Bob is a bachelor Bob is not married Sense and reference are complementary aspects of meaning

Denotation&Conotation Denotation: objective, dictionary meaning Conotation: certain aspects of meaning beyond which it explicitly names or describes Eg. Bachelor vs. spinster Bachelor (a young man, of eligible age for marriage) Spinster (an older woman who is past the society’s definition of standard age for marriage)

Mental Representation of Meaning A major psycholinguistic question *How is word knowledge stored or mentally represented in semantic memory? PROPOSALS Semantic features Mental Models Prototypes Networks

Words are bundles of semantic features According to the view, our mental representation of a Word is a bundle of semantic features. One attractive aspect of this view is that Word meaning is seen as acquired feature by feature, as opposed to all or none. This appears to provide a natural explanation for some semantic errors children make in the course of language development This view also helps us understand words within a given semantic domain. One is the domain known as verbs of possession Eg. Give, receive, pay, trade, buy, sell, spend

Prototypes One problem that confronts the theory semantic features is that it assumes that there are defining features after the words we use, but there are many words for which this is not the case (Wittgenstein, 1953) Eg. Games – board games, card games, ball games, Olympic games Wittgenstein went on to characterize these similarities as family resemblance

Prototype The category instances that appear to be the best instances of that category are referred to as prototypes. It appears that prototypes have a priviliged status in memory, as we are more adept at retrieving prototypes than other members of a category (Rosch, 1973; Rosch & Mervis, 1975; but also see Armstrong, Gleitman &Gleitman, 1983)

Networks Some words naturally cause others to spring to mind. Bread calls forth butter, table leads to chair, and dog beckons cat. These rapid, strong associations between words may obscure the more indirect and subtle connections that exist between words. For example, how are butter and smooth alike? In an attempt to describe these relationships, we might resort to a spatial metaphor and say that all words are closer to some words than to others and that the distance between words is associated with the their degree of similarity or relatedness This intuitive concept has been sharpened somewhat in the concept of a semantic network.

Networks According to this view, words are represented in memory through a rich network of sense relations. Each word concept is represented as a distinct node in the network and is connected to other words by means of various labeled relations or links. Eg. Dog is a subordinate of animal, a coordinate of cat, and a superordinate of collie.

Mental Models Johnson-Laird, Herrman, and Chaffin (1984) have criticized the notion that semantic networks are all sense and no refence. Johnson-Laird (1983) has suggested that the concept of a mental model might be fruitfully applied to the problems of reference. A mental model is an internal cognitive structure that represents some aspects of our environment. Such models are not limited to linguistic aspects. We have, for example, a model of our visual environment , in the form of a mental image, which allows us to navigate our way through our environment. If you are blindfolded and taken into a room in your house, you would probably be able to find your way around fairly well. But suppose the furniture is moved, you would have a great deal of trouble moving around.

Mental Models However, if I warned you when you were about to run into something, you woıld in short order order form vivid images of each piece of furniture in its new location (Johnson-Laird, 1988) When we hear a sentence, we may construct « a mental model of the particular state of affairs characterized by the utterance » (Johnson-Laird et al., 1984, p.311). This model can then be used to evaluate whether the sentence is true or not, by comparing the model with perceptual evidence, at least for those sentences that refer to our immediate environment.

Structure of the Internal Lexicon Experimental research that examines the organization of the lexicon The most common experimental procedure is known as a semantic verification task. ‘An apple is a fruit’ In this task, a person is presentended with a statement of the form An A is a B, such as sentence above, and asked to determine as quickly as possible whether the sentence is true or false. Since extremely few errors are made on this task since, the time taken to answer is usually what is measured. This time is thought to reflect the organization of information in the internal lexicon.

Sample items in a Semantic Verification Task A robin is a bird A butterfly is a bird A robin can fly A goose is a computer A horse is a mammal A tomato is a vegetable A mouse has teeth A monkey can read A pickle has fingernails Thomas Edison invented the telephone An octopus runs on batteries Abraham Lincoln had a beard

1. Hierarchical Network Models Network models are those that assume that our memory for word concepts forms a system of interconnected elements. A network is hierarchical if some of these elements stand above or below other members of the network. The research of Collins and Quillian (1969, 1970, 1972) stands as the prototype of this approach. A key assumption made by these researchers deals with the concept of cognitive economy. They assumed that the space available for the storage of semantic information was limited, so that some information that could conceivably be stored in more than one place would be stored only at the highest possible node.

1. Hierarchical Network Models Eg. The information that birds can breathe is stored at the animal level since it is true of all animals. Rather than store it at all nodes, the researchers suggest that we store the information just once but make it available to other nodes through the network of relations. A hierachical network model of Semantic information related to animals

1. Hierarchical Network Models In order to derive testable predictions from the model, Collins and Quillian had to make some additional assumptions about the way semantic information is retrieved. If we were presented with the sentence ‘a bird is an animal’, the sentence would activate both the bird node and the animal node. The process of deciding whether the sentence is true or false is based, according to Collins and Quillian, on a mechanism known as intersection search. They assumed that we continue to search for relevant information until the two items in the sentence intersect.

1. Hierarchical Network Models An animal is a bird In the sentence above, there would be an intersection, but a check of the relations would indicate that the sentence contradicts the information in the lexicon. Taken together, cognitive economy and intersection search yield the prediction that making decisions of the form ‘A bird is an animal’ or ‘A bird can breathe’ takes longer than deciding about ‘An animal is an animal’ or ‘An animal can breathe’ In each case, it is because we must mentally traverse one relation in the network to decide whether the statement is true or false for birds, but no relations need to be followed to determine this for animals.

1. Hierarchical Network Models The early work of Collins and Quillian and others (Landuer&Meyer, 1972) found just this relationship in the verification times. They called this the category-size effect. Formulization An A is a B or An A has B, the higher the location of B in the hierarchy in relation to A, the longer the reaction times Two major problems about the Collins and Quillian model.

Major Problems One was that the major result that supported the model, the category-size effect, was cast into doubt Conrad (1972) found that the frequency with which a property is associated with a concept is another important factor in determining response times. More importantly, when frequency is controlled, the category-size effect disappears. Eg. The statement that ‘a bird has feathers’ may be varified more rapidly than the statement ‘a bird can eat’, not because of their relative positions in a hierarchy of semantic relations but simply because ‘bird-feather’ is a more frequent combination than ‘bird-eat’

Major Problems 2nd major problem was that the original Collins and Quillian model assumed that all items on a given level of the hierarchy were more or less equal Canary and ostrich, for example, were both subordinates of bird and one link away from bird, so they should take equal time to verify. In fact, they don’t. It seems that this is generally true; some instances of categories are usually verified faster than others.

Typicality effect Smith, Shoben, and Rips (1974) examined the effect of category similarity on verification times and concluded that; Similarity reduces verification times for true statements and increases it for false statements Eg. A robin is a bird An ostrich is a bird A whale is a fish A horse is a fish 1 takes less time than 2; moreover, 3 takes longer than 4

Typicality effect This has generally been called the typicality effect: items that are more typical of a given superordinate takes less time to verify than atypical items in true statements; the opposite is true for false statements. What we know about words such as ‘dog’ is not only where they fit into an overall classificatory scheme but also reflects aspects of the word that pertain more directly to its everyday use.

2. Semantic Feature Models The Smith, Shoben, and Rips model They distinguish between two types of semnatic features: defining and characteristic. A defining feature is one that must be present for an instance to be a member of the concept. Characteristic features are those that are not necessary for category membership, but are nonetheless associated with the Word. Eg. Two defining features for birds are that they must have feathers and must be animate. A characteristic feature is that a bird can sing.

2. Semantic Feature Models The model assumes that; semantic verification decisions are made by a two-stage process. At the first stage, all features of the subject and predicate term are retrieved and compared to drive an overall estimate of the similarity of the two terms. If they are highly similar, we respond true. If they are dissimilar, we respond false. If the degree of similarity is moderate, we go to a second stage, in which only defining features are considered.

Problems with the Model It relies heavily on the distinction between defining and characteristic features; Difficult to know determine what is a defining feature of any object Functional concepts, such as ‘game’ may not have any defining features at all (Wittgenstein, 1953) McCloskey (1980) argued that the familiarity of the category pairs, not the similarity, is the most significant factor in the study by Smith et al. Not yet clear whether they can be extended to larger units of language such as sentences or discourse

3. Spreading Activation Model Makes use of Collins and Quillian’s model and the notion that concepts are stored as interconnected links is retained, but The view that all such relations are equal is revised by assuming that some nodes are more accesible than others and that the degree of accesibility is related to factors such as frequency of usage and typicality. The concept of cognitive economy is also modified Collins and Loftus distinguish between a strong version of cognitive economy and a weak version. The strong version state that all properties are stored just once in the network. This claim is inconsistent with the research of Conrad (1972) the weak vrsion of cognitive economy is that such properties are still not stored in other portions of the internal lexicon where they are also applicable

3. Spreading Activation Model The process by which semantic information is retrieved is also revised Instead of an intersection search throughout the network, Collins and Loftus argue that retrieval occurs by a process of spreading activation: activation begins at a single node and then spreads in parallel form throughout the network. They assume that there is a threshold for the activation of a given node and that the node becomes active when a variety influences accumulate to a level beyond this threshold.

3. Spreading Activation Model (From ‘‘Language Production: Grammatical Encoding,’’ by K. Bock and W. Levelt. In M. A. Gernsbascher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics, p. 951. Copyright & 1994 Academic Press. Reprinted by permission.)

3. Spreading Activation Model Can also eplain the context effect, in which the natüre of the filler items influences the verification times of interest. Eg. 1. Robbery is a crime 2. Murder is a crime 3. Libel is a crime McCloskey and Gluckber (1979) found the reaction times to 1 were faster when 2 was used as a filler item than when 3 was used. This is presumably because the activation of ‘murder’ has spread to the similar item robbery.

Evaluation of the Collins and Loftus Model Compared with the earlier model, this approach is more realistic, less rigid, and more complicated. It specifically recognizes that there is a diversity of information in a semantic network and realistically assesses the complexity of semantic decision making by presenting a series of decison procedures instead of a single one BUT, it leads to a less testable model

Lexical Access WORD FREQUENCY The process of accessing or retrieving lexical information from memory is influenced by a number of factors; Frequency of the Word Its morphological structure Whether a semantically related Word has just been encountered Whether or not the Word is ambigious

Word frequency Phoneme-monitorin study by Foss (1969). Subjects listen for a continuous speech passage and do two things: comprehend the passage and listen and listen for a target phoneme such as /b/ In some instances, the target phoneme followed a high frequency Word, and in other instances, it followed a low-frequency Word. The results were clear-cut: monitoring times increased slightly after a low-frequency Word. Foss interpreted this result as evidence of an increasing processing load for low-frequency words due to the difficulty associated with acessing them.

Word frequency Lexical decision tasks: the subject sees a string of letters and must decide whether the string is a Word or not. A number of studies have shown that frequency influences response time in given tasks, with higher frequency words having shorter times (Rubenstein, Garfield, &Milliken, 1970; Whaley, 1978) Rayner and Duffy (1986) have found that Word frequency also plays a role in normal reading. They measured eye fixations to words during reading and found that low-frequency words were fixated for about 80 milliseconds longer than high-frequency words.

Word frequency Two approaches Morton’s model Forster’ approach Each Word in the lexicon is represented as logogen, which specifies the Word’s various attributes (semantic, phonological, and son on) ıt is activated by sensory input and by contextual information Each time a Word is faced, the threshold for that logogen is lowered With high frequency words, the recovery from the lowering of the threshold is less complete Forster’ approach Information about a Word is organized into a master file, and access to this info occurs through a series of Access files, one for orthographic, one for phonological info. Word are stored in these files according to the frequency

Morphological Structure From a processing stanpoint, it would make sense to distinguish between affixes (prefixes and suffixes) of a Word and the base or root Word. Morphological and base Word information are organized separately in the mental lexicon Eg. ‘Decision’ would be stored as the base Word ‘decide’ It achieves some storage economy since we wouldn’t have to store all the various forms of a Word but only the base and the set of morphemes used throughout the language

Morphological Structure Evidence for the independent storage of base Word and morpheme by Mac Kay (1978) He found that the time taken to make these responses varied with the derivational complexity -ment is linguistically simpler than –ence, which in turn is simpler than –ion. The suffix –ion is most complex because the shift from a verb to noun involves an alternation of vowels. (go-vern-ment) – (ex-is-tence) Mackay found that the times taken to produce words such as government, existence, and decision reflected their linguistic complexity

Morphological Structure Taft and Forster (1975) have drawn similar conclusions. First morphological components, then the base Word A prefixed Word goes through an initial prefix-stripping stage After this process, search for base Word is undertaken Final stage compares the prefix and the base to see if compatible Taft (1981) found that Lexical decision times were shorter for prefixed words (remind) than for words with ‘pseudoprefixes’ (relish) Rubin et al. (1979) found a difference in lexical decision times between prefixed and pseudoprefixed words only when the stimulus list contained 50% prefixed words. When 10% , no difference Process of analyzing a Word into its morphological components depends to some extent on the frequency of occurences of various types of words.

Semeantic Priming occurs when a Word presented earlier reduces the threshold for accessing a second Word. Priming has two phases Priming stimulus is presented Second stimulus (target) is presented. The subject makes some response to it and the time taken is recorded Name a Word or the string is a Word or not The times to respond to the target in the priming condition are then compared to a condition in which no priming stimulus was presented Eg. By Meyer and Schvaneveldt (1971). They used a lexical decision task and found that the time needed to classify the target ‘butter’ as a Word varied with the priming stimulus. (bread vs. nurse)

Lexical Ambiguity The form of ambiguity in which a single Word may be interpreted to have more than one meaning is referred to as lexical ambiguity. Common research questions Do ambiguous words have more than one node or logogen in semantic memory? Do we consider multiple meanings of ambiguous words when we hear or see one? How might the sentence context influence how lexically ambiguous words are processed?

Lexical Ambiguity Eg. Rapid righting with his uninjured hand saved from loss the content of the capsized canoe. Most people hear the second Word as writing, presumably because it is a more common meaning. Moreover, there is nothing in the sentence that refutes this interpretation until we get to the end. Foss (1970) was the first to apply the phoneme-monitoring technique to the study of lexical ambiguity. Eg. The man started to drill before the truck arrived The response times to monitor the first phoneme of the very next Word (here, the /b/ in before) increased slightly after an ambiguous word

Lexical Ambiguity Cairns and Kamerman (1975) extended this result. They varied the time between the ambiguous Word and the phoneme that was to be monitored and found that the increased processing load associated with lexical ambiguity was very short lived. If the phoneme was delayed by as little as two syllables, the result disappeared.

ROLE OF FREQUENCY OF MEANING Lexical Ambiguity ROLE OF FREQUENCY OF MEANING One factor that seems to be important in any discussion of lexical ambiguity is the relative dominance or frequency of usage of various Word meanings. Some words have multiple meanings that, in the absence of any biasing context, are roughly equivalent in commonality. In other instances, one meaning is clearly dominant over the others. It makes sense to assume that, all other factors being equal, common meanings should be easier to access than uncommon meanings. Hogaboam and Perfetti (1975)constructed sentences with ambiguous words in which either the primary or the secondary meaning of the word was appropriate

Lexical Ambiguity The jealous husband read the letter The antique typewriter was missing a letter They gave subjects a series of sentences such as the above and asked them to decide whether the final word was ambiguous. Decision times were faster when the sentence required the secondary sense than when it required the primary meaning. Counterintuitive, but Suppose the various meanings of an ambiguous Word are stored in separate locations in the lexicon. In this task, we must not only find the the primary meaning but also consider whether it has a less common meaning. Presumably the common meaning is easily activated, so the time taken to find the other meaning is more directly related to response time in the task.

Lexical Ambiguity Hogaboam and Perfetti say these results are consistent with both the active search and spread,ng activation views of the internal lexicon According to active search view, more common or frequent meanings are stored higher in a list of meanings associated with that Word. So, if an active search process begins at the top of the list, common meanings are more easily recovered. Alternatively, the different meanings of an ambiguous Word may have separate logogens in the lexicon (Morton, 1979) The contextual information in the sentence should serve to prime one meaning more than another. As a result, frequency of meaning and contextual information combine to produce a pattern of activation of the various meanings that a word has (Simpson, 1984).

Lexical Ambiguity ROLE OF PRIOR CONTEXT What might happen if the biasing context occured before an ambiguous word? There is evidence that we activate multiple meanings of lexically ambiguous words (Cairns & Kamerman, 1975) Can a prior semantic context override this process? Can a context that is biased toward one or another meaning of an ambiguous Word selectively activate the appropriate meaning? Top-down vs bottom-up process in language comprehension Top-down refers to contextual effects on the perception of lexical items, whereas bottom-up refers to multiple activation of even inappropriate Word meanings. Whether we activate inappropriate word meanings even when there is contextual reason not to do so?

Lexical Ambiguity Swinney (1979) examined this question with a cross-modal lexical decision task. Subjects listened to sentences containing lexical ambiguities in strongly biasing contexts. At the same time, they performed a lexical decision task on visually presented letter strings. Some of these letter strings were sementically related to one of the meanings of the ambigious word. Eg. Rumor had it that, for years, the government building has been plagued with problems. The man was not surprised when he found several spiders, roaches, and other bugs in the corner of his room. Here the ambiguous word is bug, and the biasing context favors the insect meaning over the espionage meaning

Lexical Ambiguity As the subjects heard the word bug, they saw a contextualy related Word (ant), a contextually inappropriate Word (spy), or an unrelated Word (sew). Swinney found that lexical decision times for visual words related to either meaning of the ambiguous word were shorter than for unrelated words when the visual words immediately followed the ambiguity. When the visual words were presented four syllables after the ambiguity, only the contextually appropriate meaning was facilitated These results suggest even in the presence of a strong biasing context, multiple meanings meanings of ambiguous words are briefly activated.

Review Questions Why is there more to word meaning than reference? Distinguish between denotation and connotation What semantic elements are involved in the meaning of pay and trade? Why is the Notion of family resemblances a problem for the concept of defining features? What are the advantages and disadvantages of storing redundant information, such as A bird can breathe, in a semantic network? What is a mental model? How does typicality influence semantic verification times in opposite ways for true and false statements? What evidence suggests that we store the morphemes in a multimorphemic word as separate units in memory? Why are we not likely to carry more than one meaning of an ambiguous word throughout a long sentence?

Thought Questions 1. Analyze a television game show using the concepts from this chapter. What aspects of meaning are being utilized? How are they accessed? 2. Try giving sentences like those listed on page 110, or others of your own choice, to a friend. What responses did you get, and what can you conclude from them? 3. Do you think that a fluent bilingual would have two internal lexicons, one for each language, or would there be a single lexicon? Explain your decision. 4. How might a child acquire the internal lexicon discussed in this chapter? How might the child’s linguistic experience assist in the development of the lexicon?

Reference Carroll, D. W. (1994). Psychology of Language. Brooks/Cole Publishing: California. Carroll, D. W. (2004). Psychology of Language. Thomson Wadsworth: Belmont. Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in the Mind. Blackwell Publishing: Oxford.