Group Analyses in fMRI Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
OverviewOverview Motion correction Smoothing kernel Spatial normalisation Standard template fMRI time-series Statistical Parametric Map General Linear.
Advertisements

Topographic Maps.
Intersubject Normalization for Group Analyses in fMRI Last Update: January 18, 2012 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2010, University.
Opportunity to Participate EEG studies of vision/hearing/decision making – takes about 2 hours Sign up at – Keep checking.
Anatomy What is the difference between Structural Anatomy and Functional Anatomy? What roles do each play in our understanding of the brain?
Structural and Functional Imaging This is a Functional MRI Image !?
A Brief Intro to Cortical Neuroanatomy
Guidelines to Publishing in IO Journals: A US perspective Lois Tetrick, Editor Journal of Occupational Health Psychology.
Surface-based Analysis: Intersubject Registration and Smoothing
Brain Areas and Topography Last Update: January 2011 Last Course: Psychology 9224, W2011, University of Western Ontario Jody.
Localization & Normalization
A Brief Intro to Cortical Neuroanatomy. 14 Major Sulci Main sulci are formed early in development Fissures are really deep sulci Typically continuous.
1 of 40 Lecture 4-Neuroanatomy Walter Schneider When we talk about the brain we need to be able to identify and communicate clearly on what part of the.
Group Analyses in fMRI Last Update: November 9, 2014 Last Course: Psychology 9223, F2014, Western University Jody Culham Brain.
Intersubject Normalization for Group Analyses in fMRI
Spatial Smoothing and Multiple Comparisons Correction for Dummies Alexa Morcom, Matthew Brett Acknowledgements.
Commentary + Proposal Guidelines Last Update: March 17, 2013 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2013, Western University Jody.
Accuracy, Reliability, and Validity of Freesurfer Measurements David H. Salat
Significance of Findings and Discussion
Group Averaging of fMRI Data
A Brief Intro to Cortical Neuroanatomy
BRIEF INTRO TO CORTICAL NEUROANATOMY
Experimental Psychology
Localization & Normalization
Surface-based Analysis: Inter-subject Registration and Smoothing
Topographic Maps fMRI: Theory and Practice Spring 2010.
Surface-based Analysis: Intersubject Registration and Smoothing
Surface-based Analysis: Intersubject Registration and Smoothing
The General Linear Model (GLM): the marriage between linear systems and stats FFA.
Davide Nardo, Valerio Santangelo, Emiliano Macaluso  Neuron 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Lior Shmuelof, Ehud Zohary  Neuron 
Volume 87, Issue 4, Pages (August 2015)
Michael S Beauchamp, Kathryn E Lee, Brenna D Argall, Alex Martin 
Analysis of the Neuronal Selectivity Underlying Low fMRI Signals
Detecting Gray Matter Maturation via Tensor-based Surface Morphometry
Surface-Based and Probabilistic Atlases of Primate Cerebral Cortex
Surface-Based and Probabilistic Atlases of Primate Cerebral Cortex
Delayed Striate Cortical Activation during Spatial Attention
Disruption of Large-Scale Brain Systems in Advanced Aging
Volume 35, Issue 4, Pages (August 2002)
Uri Hasson, Michal Harel, Ifat Levy, Rafael Malach  Neuron 
Hierarchical Models and
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity  Rodrigo M. Braga, Randy L. Buckner 
Visual Cortex Extrastriate Body-Selective Area Activation in Congenitally Blind People “Seeing” by Using Sounds  Ella Striem-Amit, Amir Amedi  Current.
Reward Circuitry Activation by Noxious Thermal Stimuli
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Michael S Beauchamp, Kathryn E Lee, Brenna D Argall, Alex Martin 
Negative BOLD Differentiates Visual Imagery and Perception
Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology
Lior Shmuelof, Ehud Zohary  Neuron 
Michael S. Beauchamp, Kathryn E. Lee, James V. Haxby, Alex Martin 
Uri Hasson, Orit Furman, Dav Clark, Yadin Dudai, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
Negative BOLD Differentiates Visual Imagery and Perception
Integration of Local Features into Global Shapes
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Three-Dimensional Shape Representation in Monkey Cortex
Facial-Expression and Gaze-Selective Responses in the Monkey Amygdala
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Michael S. Beauchamp, Kathryn E. Lee, James V. Haxby, Alex Martin 
MultiModality Registration using Hilbert-Schmidt Estimators
Volume 47, Issue 6, Pages (September 2005)
Cortical Mechanisms Specific to Explicit Visual Object Recognition
Introduction to Econometrics, 5th edition
César F. Lima, Saloni Krishnan, Sophie K. Scott 
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Presentation transcript:

Group Analyses in fMRI Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute Department of Psychology Western University http://www.fmri4newbies.com/ Group Analyses in fMRI Last Update: November 21, 2016 Last Course: Psychology 9223, F2016, Western University

Group Analyses 1. Get all the subjects’ brains into a common space Talairach space MNI space cortex-based alignment 2A. Do group statistics Random Effects GLM and/or 2B. Use a Region of Interest Approach

Combining Group Data

Brains are Heterogeneous Slide from Duke course

Talairach Coordinate System Talairach & Tournoux, 1988 made an atlas based on one brain any brain can be squished or stretched to fit hers and locations can be described using a 3D coordinate system (x, y, z) … from an alcoholic old lady Note: That’s TalAIRach, not TAILarach!

Rotate brain into ACPC plane Find anterior commisure (AC) Corpus Callosum Fornix Find posterior commisure (PC) ACPC line = horizontal axis Pineal Body “bent asparagus” Note: official Tal says to use top of AC and bottom of PC but I suspect few people actually do this Source: Duvernoy, 1999

AC = Origin x = 0 + y = 0 z z = 0 - L R + y - - + x

Left is what?!!! L R R L Neurologic (i.e. sensible) convention left is left, right is right L R x = 0 - + Note: Make sure you know what your magnet and software are doing before publishing left/right info! Radiologic (i.e. stupid) convention left is right, right is left R L

Tip Put a vitamin E capsule on the one side of the subject’s head or coil.

Deform brain into Talairach space Mark 8 points in the brain: anterior commisure posterior commisure front back top bottom (of temporal lobe) left right Squish or stretch brain to fit in “shoebox” of Tal system ACPC=0 y>0 y<0 z y AC=0 y>0 y<0 x (left < 0; right > 0) Extract 3 coordinates

Talairach Tables Source: Culham et al., 2003, Exp. Brain Res. Talairach coordinates can be useful for other people to check whether their activation foci are similar to yours Often it’s easiest to just put coordinates in a table to avoid cluttering text

Do We need a “Tarailach Atras”? Variability between Japanese and European brains, both male (red > yellow > green > blue) Variability between male and female brains, both European Source: Zilles et al., 2001, NeuroImage

Talairach Pros and Cons Advantages widespread system allows averaging of fMRI data between subjects allows researchers to compare activation foci relatively easy to use Disadvantages does better for central regions of cortex, but not great for most of cortex not appropriate for all brains (e.g., group variability, patients may not fit well) ignores left- vs. right-hemisphere asymmetries activation foci can vary considerably – other landmarks like sulci may be more reliable

MNI Space Researchers at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) created a better template based on a morphed average of hundreds of brains many different versions http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/MINC/Atlases/Atlases/History

MNI Space Benefits MNI Space is based on many subjects not just one brain like Talairach space MNI transformations use nonlinear warping, which leads to better intersubject alignment Image Source

Converting Between MNI and Tal space You many want to convert between the systems Only Tal provides Brodmann’s areas Need to convert for meta-analyses The MNI and Talairach coordinates are similar but not identical e.g., temporal lobes extend 10 mm lower in MNI brain Caveat: careful comparison requires a transformation -- converters can be found online http://www.brainmap.org/icbm2tal/ Source: http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/personal/matthew.brett/abstracts/MNITal/mniposter.pdf

Anatomical Localization Sulci and Gyri gray matter (dendrites & synapses) white matter (axons) FUNDUS BANK GYRUS SULCUS CROWN pial surface gray/white border neuron SULCUS FISSURE GYRUS Source: Ludwig & Klingler, 1956, in Tamraz & Comair, 2000

Variability of Sulci Source: Szikla et al., 1977, in Tamraz & Comair, 2000

Effects of Sulcal Variability Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage

Variability of Functional Areas Watson et al., 1995 - motion-selective area, MT+ (=V5) is quite variable in stereotaxic space however, the area is quite consistent in its location relative to sulci junction of inferior temporal sulcus and lateral occipital sulcus see also Dumoulin et al., 2000

Cerebral cortex is a crumpled map

Cortical Surfaces segment gray-white matter boundary render cortical surface inflate cortical surface sulci = concave = dark gray gyri = convex = light gray

Cortical Inflation Movie Movie: unfoldorig.mpeg http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/~sereno/unfoldorig.mpg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page

Cortical Flattening Source: Brain Voyager Getting Started Guide 2) make cuts along the medial surface (Note, one cut typically goes along the fundus of the calcarine sulcus though in this example the cut was placed below) 1) inflate the brain 3) unfold the medial surface so the cortical surface lies flat 4) correct for the distortions so that the true cortical distances are preseved Source: Brain Voyager Getting Started Guide

Spherical Averaging Future directions of fMRI: Use cortical surface mapping coordinates Inflate the brain into a sphere Use sulci and/or functional areas to match subject’s data to template Cite “latitude” & “longitude” of spherical coordinates Source: Fischl et al., 1999

Spherical Averaging Source: Fischl et al., 1999 Movie: brain2ellipse.mpeg http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/~sereno/coord1.mpg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page Movie: morph-curv1.mpg http://www.cogsci.ucsd.edu/~sereno/morph-curv1.mpg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page Source: Fischl et al., 1999

Before and After CBA Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage

Before and After CBA hand motor area (M1) hand somatosensory area (S1) Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage

Gains in Overlap Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage

Voxelwise Group Analyses Fixed vs. Random Effects

Example Three subjects Three conditions: Baseline, Faces, Objects For simplicity, just consider one voxel in FFA

Stupid Way: Concatenated Fixed Effects (FFX) Concatenate data, pretend you have one subject who did three runs

Stupid Way: Concatenated Fixed Effects (FFX) Make one predictor for Faces and one for Objects (2 df) Scale predictors (by beta weights) Note why this is stupid Assumes all subject show same magnitude of activation Errors in this assumption   residuals

Better Way: Separate Subjects FFX Don’t concatenate Separate predictors for 3 subjects x 2 conditions 6 df

Problem: Separate Subjects FFX We could do business-as-usual GLM and see if predictors account for significant variance considering noise Effectively, we are asking how confident we are that this effect is true (not due to chance) in these three subjects (and only these three subjects) BUT usually, we want to generalize to the population from which we sampled

Best Way: Random Effects (RFX)

Second-level analysis Nothing too complicated… it’s effectively just a paired t-test Subject βFaces βObjects Difference βFaces - βObjects S1 0.552 0.105 0.447 S2 2.061 1.121 0.940 S3 1.019 0.247 0.772 Mean 0.719 SD 0.250 SEM [=SD/sqrt(N)] 0.145 tcrit(df=3) 4.3 95%CI lower (= mean – (t*SEM)) 0.097 95%CI lower (= mean + (t*SEM)) 1.34 Estimated Distribution of Differences does not include zero  significant (p<.05) 0.719 0.097 1.34 95% CI

Repeat for the other 60,000 voxels… Huettel, Song & McCarthy, 2008 First-level analysis Second-level analysis

If you really want to be correct… We often refer to this type of analysis as random effects (RFX) Since subjects is a random effect but other aspects (e.g., stimulus categories) are fixed effects, technically the proper term is Mixed Effects Analysis Other common jargon = Second-level Analysis

Take-home Message RFX enables us to generalize to the population from which we sampled subjects Degrees of freedom comes from number of subjects, not number of time points No need to worry about correction for serial correlations for most fMRI studies, this means underpaid graduate students in need of a few bucks!

Examples from a real data set

Concatenated FFX Example 17 Subjects x 2 runs with Faces & Houses df = 17 Ss x 2 runs/S x 264 vols/run - 1 one predictor per condition

FFX Separate Subjects … If you’re looking at pilot data from a few Ss, with RFX it will be hard to see any effects. You can do FFX with separate subjects. It has the same problems as concatenated FFX but at least enables you to examine the consistency between Ss

Now that our df no longer depends on # volumes, RFX S1 S2 S3 … S17 This contrast is just like doing a paired t-test between Faces and Objects with 17 Ss df = 17 Ss - 1 Now that our df no longer depends on # volumes, we don’t have to worry about correction for serial correlations with RFX

The Problem of Intersubject Variability The hand area lies in the “hand knob”, a section of the central sulcus that looks like an upside-down omega Primary Motor Cortex (M1) lies along the anterior bank while Primary Somatosensory Cortex (S1) lies along the posterior bank Ω Ω 2+ cm Although these areas are reliably localized with respect to sulcal landmarks within individuals, the sulcus has high anatomical variability between individuals

The Problem of Intersubject Variability The hand area lies in the “hand knob”, a section of the central sulcus that looks like an upside-down omega Primary Motor Cortex (M1) lies along the anterior bank while Primary Somatosensory Cortex (S1) lies along the posterior bank Although these areas are reliably localized with respect to sulcal landmarks within individuals, the sulcus has high anatomical variability between individuals Due to this variability, activation of the same area (e.g., the motor hand area during finger tapping) can appear scattered in stereotaxic (Talairach or MNI) space. Given that random effects (RFX) analyses require somewhat consistent effects across participants, this can handicap statistical power This also means that it can be tricky to isolate effects in one area (e.g., M1) without contamination from adjacent areas (e.g., S1) Ω Ω S1 β = 3 S2 β = 0 S3 β = 0 RFX non-sig S1 S2 S3 3 Ss superimposed

Three Solutions to Intersubject Variability RFX non-sig S1 β = 2.5 S2 β = 2.5 S3 β = 1 RFX sig Solution 1: Spatial Smoothing increase likelihood of consistent effects across Ss doesn’t solve problem that voxels likely contain combination of different areas (M1 and S1 in this case) Solution 2: Cortex-based Alignment solution works very well and makes it less likely to blur across adjacent functional areas generation of surfaces can be time-consuming (though automation is getting better) cortex-centric S1 β = 3 S2 β = 3 S3 β = 3 RFX highly sig S1 S2 S3 Solution 3: Individual ROIs identify ROI anatomically in each S (e.g., based on anterior bank of hand knob) or functionally in each S (based on functional localizer) anatomical ROIs can require manual labour functional ROIs can be somewhat subjective doesn’t work for unexpected activation sites S1 β = 3 S2 β = 3 S3 β = 3 RFX highly sig Original Data

Random Effects Analysis Brain Voyager recommends you don’t even toy with random effects unless you’ve got 10 or more subjects (and 50+ is best) Random effects analyses can really squash your data, especially if you don’t have many subjects. Though standards were lower in the early days of fMRI, today it’s virtually impossible to publish any group voxelwise data without RFX analysis

Strategies for Exploration vs. Publication Deductive approach Have a specific hypothesis/contrast planned Run all your subjects Run the stats as planned Publish Inductive approach Run a few subjects to see if you’re on the right track Spend a lot of time exploring the pilot data for interesting patterns “Find the story” in the data You may even change the experiment, run additional subjects, or run a follow-up experiment to chase the story While you need to use rigorous corrections for publication, do not be overly conservative when exploring pilot data or you might miss interesting trends Random effects analyses can be quite conservative so you may want to do exploratory analyses with fixed effects (and then run more subjects if needed so you can publish random effects)

How can we identify activation foci? Talairach coordinates Example: The FFA is at x = 40, y = -55, z = -10 Anatomical localization Example: The FFA is in the right fusiform gyrus at the level of the occipitotemporal junction Functional localization Example: The FFA includes all voxels around the fusiform gyrus that are activated by the comparison between faces and objects Kanwisher, McDermott & Chun, 1997, J Neurosci

Talairach Daemon http://www.talairach.org

Neurosynth Neurosynth (Dec. 2014)

Automated Coordinate Extraction Yarkoni et al., 2001

Example: Working Memory META- ANALYSIS from previous studies FORWARD INFERENCE What is the probability of activation here a study involving working memory? REVERSE INFERENCE If there is activation here, what is the probability the study involved working memory? Yarkoni et al., 2001

Automated Coordinate Extraction Yarkoni et al., 2001

Definition of an “Area” Neuroimager’s definition of an area: Some blob vaguely in the vicinity (+/- ~3 cm) of where I think it ought to be that lights up for something I think it ought to light up for Neuroanatomist’s definition of an area: A circumscribed region of the cerebral cortex in which neurons together serve a specific function, receive connections from the same regions, have a common structural arrangement, and in some cases show a topographic arrangement may also be called a cortical field

Cortical Fields: Multiple Criteria Function an area has a unique pattern of responses to different stimuli Architecture different brain areas show differences between cortical properties (e.g., thickness of different layers, sensitivity to various dyes) Connectivity Different areas have different patterns of connections with other areas Topography many sensory areas show topography (retinotopy, somatotopy, tonotopy) boundaries between topographic maps can indicate boundaries between areas (e.g., separate maps of visual space in visual areas V1 and V2)

Can We Use Multiple Criteria in Human Imaging? Function this is often the only criterion in fMRI Architecture there are now probabilistic maps of human brain areas available (Zilles lab) Connectivity DTI and functional connectivity now give us options here Topography topography is useful in imaging, especially for early and mid-level sensory areas

Brodmann Area 17 Meets 21st Century Anatomical MRI Functional MRI Layer 4 Logothetis fMRI data: image from http://www.bruker-biospin.com/imaging_neuroanatomy.html Goense, Zappe & Logothetis, 2007, MRI Layer 4 fMRI activation (0.3 x 0.3 x 2 mm spin echo)

Brodmann’s Areas Brodmann (1905): Based on cytoarchitectonics: study of differences in cortical layers between areas Most common delineation of cortical areas More recent schemes subdivide Brodmann’s areas into many smaller regions Monkey and human Brodmann’s areas not necessarily homologous

Brodmann Area 17

Retinotopic Maps EXPANDING RINGS ROTATING WEDGES

DTI in V1 Saentz & Fine, 2010, NeuroImage

Maps, Maps, Maps … even in parietal lobe … … even in frontal lobe … Hagler & Sereno, 2006, NeuroImage Wandell et al., 2007, Neuron

Other Sensory “-topies” Touch: Somatotopy Servos et al., 1998 red = wrist; orange = shoulder Audition: Tonotopy Sylvian fissure temporal lobe cochlea Movie: tonotopy.mpeg http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/~sereno/downsweep2.mpg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page

Learning Brain Anatomy Duvernoy, 1999, The Human Brain: Surface, Blood Supply, and Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy beautiful pictures good schematic diagrams clear anatomy slices of real brain Springer, US$439 DISCONTINUED Ono, 1990, Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci great for showing intersubject variability gives probabilities of configurations and stats on sulci Theime, US$199 Damasio,2005, Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images, 2nd edition good for showing sulci across wide range of slice planes 2nd edition much better than 1st edition Oxford University Press, US$100 Tamraz & Comair, 2000, Atlas of Regional Anatomy of the Brain Using MRI with Functional Correlations good overview Springer, US$203 Talairach & Tournoux, 1988. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain just because it’s the standard doesn’t mean it’s good Theime, US$240

Brain Tutor Mac/PC: free iOS App: $1.99

Proposal Guidelines Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute Department of Psychology Western University http://www.fmri4newbies.com/ Proposal Guidelines Last Update: November 21, 2016 Last Course: Psychology 9223, F2016, Western University

Research Proposal Due December 12, 2016 Goals give students an opportunity to demonstrate what they’ve learned and apply ideas to their research area give students practice in writing grants/papers 16 double-spaced pages + figures must be original not thesis not something your advisor totally worked out for you can get some suggestions from advisor but core of proposal should be your work

Research Proposal partially like a grant partially like a paper proposal for experiment make case for why experiment should be done “hasn’t been done before” is not good enough clear question, hypotheses conclusion: so what? immunization against potential criticism partially like a paper just one experiment, not 5 years of experiments in-depth methods be clear about design (e.g., protocol) and analyses be clear about contrasts Appendix with budget and time line don’t worry about formatting – spend your time on content not formatting

Range of Approaches Standard univariate fMRI with hypothesis-driven GLM Block or Event-related Advanced designs e.g., MVPA Data-driven fMRI e.g., ICA on resting state data Approaches we’ve touched on in class e.g., intersubject correlations Anatomical approaches DTI For the more computationally inclined better ways to analyze data if you must use equations, explain them intuitively in text (and consider putting them in an appendix) Be aware that we won’t discuss these in too much detail in class; therefore you would need to have some prior exposure or to do some extra reading

Two questions to consider whenever you write a paper or give a talk Who is my audience? a math-phobic professor who will be checking whether you understood the core ideas of fMRI What is my goal? show professor that you can find a way to use neuroimaging in your research show professor that you understand jargon and concepts Bonus be clever and creative write clearly and concisely solidify your understanding of neuroimaging approaches think more deeply about how to apply neuroimaging

Sections Introduction Give enough information to put the research in context and lead the reader to the conclusion that the experiment you’re proposing is a reasonable next step You don’t need to cite every paper in the history of neuroimaging Do enough of a lit search to be fairly certain proposal hasn’t been done Replication attempts discouraged (but may be considered with sufficient justification) Methods Include enough detail to demonstrate that you understand jargon and key concepts Be clear about specific contrasts Results How could it turn out? May want to include graph of hypotheses Conclusions Don’t just summarize; conclude! So what?! What would it mean if the results turned out one way or another? Are there any caveats that should be acknowledged? What is the broader significance of the research? References whatever format you like (e.g., APA) don’t go overboard Figures (optional) Appendix How much will it cost? How long will it take?

Key Methods # participants (pilot + discarded + final) Design (block, event-related) Timing of TR, trials, blocks, jittering, runs, session Stimulus/task conditions + rationale Preprocessing steps include criterion for motion correction Approach (localizer?; ROI and/or voxelwise; individual and/or group; for event-related convolution or deconvolution) Contrasts don’t just say “activation for faces” or “activation for faces vs houses, bodies & scrambled” define specific contrast: e.g., “a conjunction of faces vs. each control category [(F>H)AND(F>B)AND(F>S)] corrections (esp. multiple comparisons)

Example of Hypothesis Figure