Theory in mathematics education

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 4 Key Concepts.
Advertisements

 Distributed Cognition emphasizes the distributed nature of cognitive phenomena across individuals, artifacts, and representations that are both internal.
Features of SLA In this lecture, we are going to set the main general concluding features of SLA under the question: What are the main features of SLA?
NEGOTIATION AS A TEACHING STRATEGY WHERE PARTICIPANTS HOLD AN EQUAL BALANCE OF POWER!!!! Is this possible between a young child and educator? Helen Bernstone.
What separates humans from animals? What separates advanced societies from primitive societies? What separates advanced cognition from basic cognition?
HCC class lecture 5 comments John Canny 2/2/05. Administrivia.
Lev Vygotsky A Russian psychologist (1896 – 1934) A Russian psychologist (1896 – 1934) Introduced to American audiences in the 1960’s Introduced to American.
Constructivism Constructivism — particularly in its "social" forms — suggests that the learner is much more actively involved in a joint enterprise with.
Constructivist theories of cognitive development in adolescence
Constructivism A Short Introduction David Geelan University of Alberta January 2002.
Vygotsky in the Classroom (about 14 min) The 3rd Principal Principle: Learning Occurs Best in the “Zone” Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) = gap btwn.
LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT
Click to edit Master title style  Click to edit Master text styles  Second level  Third level  Fourth level  Fifth level Click to edit Master title.
Lev Vygotsky’s Social Development Theory
Vygotsky’s Theory of Cognitive Development and scaffolding
Vygotsky: Social Learning Theory
HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1 PSYCHOLOGY 3050: Social Construction of Mind
Vygotsky: Social Cultural Learning Lev Vygotsky ( )
SOCIALIZING FORCES WORK BEST WHEN THEY ARE IN TUNE WITH INNER MATURATIONAL PRINCIPLES ARNOLD GESELL A CHILD IS THE PRODUCT OF THE ENVIRONMENT... BUT DEVELOPMENT.
“Psychology today seems less capable than ever of providing a coherent account of the human mind”
Theories of Learning Pavlov’s Classical Behaviorism: stimulus-response behavior leads to learning learning process consists of the formation of associations.
Theoretical Framework for the Teaching Proposal
Chapter 2: Cognitive Development:
Stephen Pate CEPD 8102 University of West Georgia.
C.H.I.L.D Foundation Drina Madden
EEX 4251 Developing Mathematical Knowledge Dr. Matthew C. Robinson, Spring 2008.
The Almighty Critical Look at Critical Language Teacher Education.
Vygotsky The zone of proximal development. The ZPD This was a term used by Vygotsky to refer to the distance between what a child can achieve alone, and.
COLLABORATIVE PROBLEM- SOLVING AND ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT Lala Mamedov KA702.
Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) Don Martin EPSY 6304 Cognition and Development UT-Brownsville Professor Garcia By PresenterMedia.comPresenterMedia.com.
Sociocultural Theory Week 4, “Sociocultural Approaches to Learning and Development”
Constructivism A learning theory for today’s classroom.
Educational Psychology Ch. 2 Cognitive Development and Language Ashleigh Dunn 03/19/2011.
Vygotsky in the Classroom Cultural-Historical Activity Theory in Teaching. Dr. Natalia Gajdamaschko, October 6, VCC, Vancouver, Canada. Vygotsky 1: Introduction,
HOW CHILDREN LEARN THE SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIVIST LEV VYGOTSKY.
Development of Cognition and Language: Vygotsky EDU 330: Educational Psychology Daniel Moos.
Socio-cultural theory teaching/learning centred. Historic Roots Vygotsky (1920s-1930s) Russian educational psychologist
Constructivism Prepared by: Shuhudha Rizwan (2007)
Three generations of cultural-historical activity theory?
Marcia Baxter Magolda Learners’ Narratives: Real-life Stories about Constructivist- Developmental Pedagogy Improving Student Learning.
Constructivism is a theory based on observation and scientific study about how people learn. It is a teaching philosophy based on the concept that learning.
Adolescent and : Cognitive Development: Review
Introduction to Theories in Mathematics Education
The Constructivist Theory of Learning
The Interpersonal Mode
The Dialectical Relationship between Prior and Present Learning: Mapping Vygotsky’s Activity Theory to the Prior Learning Portfolio Process Jessica Kindred,
Educational Psychology: Theory and Practice
Cultural-historical scientific school: the issues that L. S
Integrating Curriculum Through Project and Play
Developing Mathematics Teaching
What separates humans from animals?
Constructivism in the Mathematics Classroom
Piaget and Vygotsky.
CONSTRUCTIVISM: CONCEPT, PRINCIPLES & BENEFITS
TEORÍAS DEL APRENDIZAJE
PSYB3 – Vygotsky's theory of Cognitive Development
Situated Cognitive Theory
Teaching /Learning / ICT
Development of Cognition and Language: Vygotsky
KNOWLEDGE AND MEANING ARE CONSTRUCTED BY THE INDIVIDUAL
CS7067 E-Learning: Principles & Practice II
Psychological theories of learning and instruction
Liliam, Meagan and Pazit
The Influence of Teachers’ Technology Use on Instructional Practices
Constructivism Constructivism — particularly in its "social" forms — suggests that the learner is much more actively involved in a joint enterprise with.
Doctoral Students as Co-Teachers in Graduate Courses: An Application of Apprenticeships in Graduate Education Katherine McKee, Agricultural & Extension.
Sociocultural Theory by Lev Vygotsky
CONSTRUCTIVISM Submitted To: Ma’am Misbah Yasmeen BPGCW (Air University)
LEARNER-CENTERED PSYCHOLOGICAL PRINCIPLES. The American Psychological Association put together the Leaner-Centered Psychological Principles. These psychological.
Presentation transcript:

Theory in mathematics education A sketch of key ideas and concepts

Overview Constructivism Sociocultural theories Social practice theory Naïve, Radical & Social Communication and Intersubjectivity Sociocultural theories Vygotsky – general law of cultural development Mediation Spontaneous and scientific concepts Instruction and ZPD Social practice theory Activity theory Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Constructivism von Glasersfeld, Steffe, Cobb, Confrey Naïve constructivism Radical constructivism Making sense /Cognition Individual cogniser/constructor Clinical interviews/Classroom experiments Social constructivism Intersubjectivity First and second-order models Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

2 principles of Radical Constructivism Constructivism is a theory of knowledge with roots in philosophy, psychology, and cybernetics. It asserts two main principles whose application has far reaching consequences for the study of cognitive development and learning, as well as for the practice of teaching, psychotherapy and interpersonal management in general. The two principles are: 1. Knowledge is not passively received but actively built up by the cognising subject; 2a The function of cognition is adaptive, in the biological sense of the term, tending towards fit or viability; 2b Cognition serves the subjects’ organisation of the experiential world, not the discovery of ontological reality. (von Glasersfeld 1987; 1990; Cited in Jaworski, 1994) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Post epistemological If experience is the only contact a knower can have with the world, there is no way of comparing the products of experience with the reality from which whatever messages we receive are supposed to emanate. The question, how veridical the acquired knowledge might be, can therefore not be answered. To answer it one would have to compare what one knows with what exists in the ‘real’ world – and to do that one would have to know what ‘exists’. The paradox then is to assess the truth of your knowledge before you come to know it. (Glasersfeld 1983) Radical constructivism is thus radical because it breaks with convention and delivers a theory of knowledge in which knowledge does not reflect an ‘objective’ ontological reality, but exclusively an ordering and organisation of a world constituted by our experience (Glasersfeld 1984) (Cited in Jaworski, 1994 p. 17) Noddings (1990) suggests that Radical Constructivism is post-epistemological Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Communication Teachers and students are viewed as active meaning makers who continually give contextually based meanings to each others’ words and actions as they interact. The mathematical structures that the teacher ‘sees out there’ are considered to be the product of his or her own conceptual activity. From this perspective mathematical structures are not perceived, intuited, or taken in but are constructed by reflectively abstracting from the reorganising sensorimotor and conceptual activity. They are inventions of the mind. Consequently the teacher who points to mathematical structures is consciously reflecting on mathematical objects that he or she had previously constructed. Because the teachers and students each construct their own meanings for words and events in the context of the on-going interaction, it is readily apparent why communication often breaks down, why teachers and students frequently talk past each other. The constructivist’s problem is to account for successful communication . (Cobb, 1998 cited in Jaworski 1994) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Intersubjectivity Intersubjectivity is a state where each participant in a socially-ongoing interaction feels assured that others involved in then interaction think pretty much as he or she imagines they do. … Intersubjectivity is not a claim of identical thinking. … Rather it is a claim that each person sees no reason to believe others think differently than he or she presumes they do. Thompson (ref??) First and second-order models See also Jaworski, 1994, pp.208-212 Compare with intersubjectivity as shared meanings and values within social settings. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

The third principle: social constructivism Recognition of the social constructing of knowledge through its negotiation and mediation with others: The third principle derives from the sociology of knowledge, and acknowledges that reality is constructed intersubjectively, that is it is socially negotiated between significant others who are able to share meanings and social perspectives of a common lifeworld (Berger & Luckmann, 1966). This principle acknowledges the sociocultural and socioemotional contexts of learning, highlights the central role of language in learning, and identifies the learner as an interactive co-constructor of knowledge . (Taylor & Campbell-Williams, 1993; cited in Jaworski, 1994, p. 24) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Sociocultural theories Wertsch, Cole, Rogoff, Lerman Culture and cognition Language & discourse Community Participation Mediation Situated cognition Distributed cognition Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Vygotsky Human learning presupposes a special social nature and a process by which children grow into the intellectural life of those around them. (1978, p.88) Every function of a child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level, and later, on the individual level; first between people (interpsychological), and then inside the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of concepts. All the higher functions originate as actual relations between human individuals. (1978, p. 57. Emphasis in original.) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Mediation Vygotsky emphasized the importance in social participation, or action, of the mediation of tools or signs. Wertsch (1991), following “the tradition of the theory of activity proposed by A. N. Leont’ev”, refers to “goal-directed action” and writes, “human action typically employs ‘mediational means’ such as tools and language”. As social beings we use tools (physical) or signs (intellectual) to enable us to achieve the objects or goals of our activity. One of the most important tools, or signs, is language. We use language both to express meaning and to make sense of the activity in which we engage. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Based on Vygotsky’s model of a complex mediated act MEDIATING ARTEFACTS SUBJECT OBJECT OUTCOME Based on Vygotsky’s model of a complex mediated act Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

individual(s)-acting-with-mediational-means Wertsch (1991) emphasises that “the relationship between action and mediational means is so fundamental that it is more appropriate, when referring to the agent involved, to speak of ‘individual(s)-acting-with-mediational-means’ than to speak simply of ‘individual(s)’” (p. 12). Leont’ev writes, “in a society, humans do not simply find external conditions to which they must adapt their activity. Rather these social conditions bear with them the motives and goals of their activity, its means and modes. In a word, society produces the activity of the individuals it forms” (1979, pp. 47-48). Wertsch contrasts two ideas: rather than seeing mental functioning as deriving from participation in social life, he suggests that the specific structures and processes of intramental processing can be traced to their genetic precursors on the intermental plane (p. 27). In other words, individual mental thought processes and functioning have their origins fundamentally in social interaction. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Spontaneous and scientific concepts Spontaneous concepts Learning through participation in social endeavour Scientific concepts Needing pedagogical mediation for their appropriation (Schmittau, 2003, p.226) Vygotsky 1986, pp 146-209 Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Zone of proximal development ZPD is ‘an account of how the more competent assist the young and less competent to reach the higher ground from which to reflect more abstractly about the nature of things (Bruner, 1985). The ZPD is the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance, or in collaboration with more capable peers. (Vygotsky, 1978, pp. 84-7) Thus the notion of a [ZPD] enables us to propound a new formula, namely that the only ‘good learning’ is that which is in advance of development (p.89). Bruner: scaffolding, vicarious consciousness Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Social practice theory Lave & Wenger, Wenger, Adler Situated cognition Community of practice Knowledge in practice Learning in practice Belonging and becoming Participation Legitimate peripheral participation Reification Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Community of practice The term ‘community’ designates a group of people identifiable by who they are in terms of how they relate to each other, their common activities and ways of thinking, beliefs and values. Activities are likely to be explicit, whereas ways of thinking, beliefs and values are more implicit. Wenger (1998, p. 5) describes community as “a way of talking about the social configurations in which our enterprises are defined as worth pursuing and our participation is recognisable as competence”. In a learning community, “learning involves transformation of participation in collaborative endeavour” (Rogoff, 1996, p. 388). The concept of practice connotes doing, but not just doing in and of itself. It is doing in a historical and social context that gives structure and meaning to what we do. In this sense practice is always social practice (Wenger, 1998, p.47). Mathematics as a social practice (Hemmi) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Legitimate peripheral participation L&W 1991 The newcomer joining the practice and being (in a legitimate way) on its periphery Masters, or old-stagers, being the ones who define the practice and perpetuate it. Newcomers working towards centrality in the practice. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Participation and reification According to Wenger in a community of practice we have Mutual engagement: doing things together, relationships, social complexity, community maintenance Joint enterprise: mutual accountability, interpretations, rhythms, local response Shared repertoire: stories, artefacts, tools, actions, discourses, historical events, concepts (1998, p. 72ff) Learning involves participation and reification – “dual modes of existence through time” (p. 87). Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Belonging to a community of practice Identity in engagement, imagination and alignment (Wenger, 1998) Normal desirable states (Brown & McIntyre, 1993) Critical alignment (Jaworski 2006) Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Activity theory Leontev, Davidov, Engeström, Mellin Olsen Activity Mediation Tools and signs, artefacts Vygotsky’s mediational triangle Leont’ev’s Motives and goals Expanded mediational triangle Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Motives and goals According to Leont’ev, “Activity is the non-additive, molar unit of life … it is not a reaction, or aggregate of reactions, but a system with its own structure, its own internal transformations, and its own development” (p. 46). He proposed a three tiered explanation of activity. First, human activity is always energised by a motive. Second, the basic components of human activity are the actions that translate activity motive into reality, where each action is subordinated to a conscious goal. Activity can be seen as comprising actions relating to associated goals. Thirdly, operations are the means by which an action is carried out, and are associated with the conditions under which actions take place. Leont’ev’s three tiers or levels can be summarised as: activity  motive; actions  goals; operations  conditions. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Activity (System) & Motive Actions & Goals Operations & Conditions Developmental research, whose motive is to study developmental processes and, simultaneously, promote development in the learning and teaching of mathematics. Asking researchable questions, collecting and analysing data leading to findings or outcomes related to new knowledge and/or practice. Making methodological decisions related to principled and effective ways of collecting and analysing data to address research questions. The LCM project as community of inquiry, with motive to provide the environment and modes of action for teaching development to be realised. Creating opportunities for working together and engaging in inquiry to achieve a working community with practical knowledge of inquiry processes. Teachers and didacticians working in groups in workshops on mathematical problems to exemplify inquiry processes and develop common understandings. Working together on mathematics as a common area of interest and purpose in order to learn more about inquiry processes and their application in classrooms Designing mathematical tasks that will be accessible across a spectrum of mathematical experience and relatable to teaching at a range of levels Assembling a set of resources (e.g., books, software, ideas) and possibilities for translating these into usable forms in the workshop environment and beyond Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

Engeström’s ’complex model of an activity system’ MEDIATING ARTEFACTS SUBJECT OBJECT OUTCOME RULES COMMUNITY DIVISION OF LABOUR Engeström’s ’complex model of an activity system’ Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4

References Brown, S. & McIntyre, D.: 1993, Making Sense of Teaching, Open University Press, Buckingham Bruner, J. S. (1985). Vygotsky: A historical and conceptual perspective. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.) Culture Communication and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Engeström, Y. (1999). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen & R-L Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 19-38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Jaworski, B. (1994). Investigating mathematics teaching. London: Falmer Press. Jaworski B. (2006). Theory and practice in mathematics teaching development: Critical inquiry as a mode of learning in teaching. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 9(2) pp. xx-xx Jaworski, B., & Goodchild, S.: 2006, ‘Inquiry community in an activity theory frame’, in Proceedings of the 30th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Charles University, Prague,v. 3, pp.353-360.Lave, J.: 1996 ‘Teaching as Learning, in Practice’, in Mind Culture and Activity, 3(3), pp. 149-164. Lave, J. & Wenger, E.: 1991, Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA. Leont’ev, A. N. (1979). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 37-71). New York: M. E. Sharpe. Rogoff, B., Matusov, E. and White, C.: 1996, ‘Models of Teaching and Learning: Participation in a community of learners’, in D. R. Olson and N. Torrance (eds.), The Handbook of Education and Human Development, Blackwell, Oxford, pp.388-414. Schmittau, J.: 2003: ‘Cultural-Historical Theory and Mathematics Education’, in A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. S. Ageyev and S. M. Miller, (eds.), Vygotsky’s Educational Theory in Cultural Context, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp.225-245. Thompson, P. (2002). Didactic objects and didactic models in radical constructivism. In K. Gravemeier, R. Lehrer, B. van Oers, & L Verschaffel (Eds)., Symbolizing. Modeling and Tool Use in Mathematics Education. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Vygotsky, L. S.: 1978, Mind in society, Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA. Vygotsky, L. S. (1986) Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Barbara Jaworski MEC 2008 4