1Department of Experimental Psychology

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
IELTS and the Academic Reading Construct Tony Green Cyril Weir Centre for Research in English Language Learning and Assessment The researchers would like.
Advertisements

Psycholinguistic what is psycholinguistic? 1-pyscholinguistic is the study of the cognitive process of language acquisition and use. 2-The scope of psycholinguistic.
The Role of Competition in Repetition Blindness Mary L. Still Alison L. MorrisIowa State University The Role of Competition in Repetition Blindness Mary.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Design If the context change theory explains directed forgetting, children should have no problem intentionally forgetting objects through a mental context.
Translation Equivalence Enhances Cross-Linguistic Syntactic Priming Sofie Schoonbaert 1, Robert Hartsuiker 1, and Martin Pickering 2 1 Ghent University,
Highly Fluent, Balanced Bilingualism Does Not Enhance Executive Function Oliver Sawi 1,2, Jack Darrow 1, Hunter Johnson 1, Kenneth Paap 1 ; 1 San Francisco.
Phonetic Similarity Effects in Masked Priming Marja-Liisa Mailend 1, Edwin Maas 1, & Kenneth I. Forster 2 1 Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing.
Working Memory: The Feature Model Presented by: Umer Fareed.
Putting Together the Pieces: Meaning Matters in Children’s Plural Comprehension Craig Van Pay, Areanna Lakowske & Jennifer Zapf.
Syntactic Priming in Bilinguals: Effects of verb repetition in an L2-monolingual and cross-lingual setting Sofie Schoonbaert 1, Robert Hartsuiker 1, &
We investigated the effort associated with incidental- and intentional-memory encoding of repeated human faces (Experiment 1) and car fronts (Experiment.
The Effects of Increased Cognitive Demands on the Written Discourse Ability of Young Adolescents Ashleigh Elaine Zumwalt Eastern Illinois University.
The Contribution of Perceptual Mechanisms to the Spacing Effect Jason Arndt & Julie Dumas Middlebury College Abstract Recent explanations of the spacing.
Second Language Proficiency Places Cognitive Constraints on Sentence Processing Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology The Pennsylvania State University.
Influence of Word Class Proportion on Cerebral Asymmetries for High and Low Imagery Words Christine Chiarello 1, Connie Shears 2, Stella Liu 3, and Natalie.
Assessment Of Mental Status By Dr. Hanan Said Ali
Wilson, “The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory” Wilson’s thesis: Items held in short-term verbal memory are encoded in an “articulatory” format.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Naming Obligatory and Optional Verbs in Aphasia Jennifer Austin and Susan T. Jackson University of Kansas,
Experimental study of morphological priming: evidence from Russian verbal inflection Tatiana Svistunova Elizaveta Gazeeva Tatiana Chernigovskaya St. Petersburg.
Participants and Procedure  Twenty-five older adults aged 62 to 83 (M = 70.86, SD = 5.89).  Recruited from St. John’s and surrounding areas  56% female.
Jelena Mirković and Maryellen C. MacDonald Language and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction How to Study Subject-Verb.
Neurological models explaining ”the concreteness effect” Christian Kastén 3. The context-availability model Its basic propositions are that: Abstract and.
References Arndt, J. & Hirshman, E. (1998). True and false recognition in MINERVA2: Explanation from a global matching perspective. Journal of Memory and.
BY DR ZAINAB ABDULZAEEZ UMAR DEPARTMENT OF FAMILY MEDICINE AMINU KANO TEACHING HOSPITAL.
The Influence of Feature Type, Feature Structure and Psycholinguistic Parameters on the Naming Performance of Semantic Dementia and Alzheimer’s Patients.
Paradoxical False Memory for Objects After Brain Damage Stephanie M. McTighe 1,2 ; Rosemary A. Cowell 3, Boyer D. Winters 4, Timothy J. Bussey 1,2 and.
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
The Critical Period for Language Acquisition: Evidence from Second Language Learning CATHERINE E. SNOW AND MARIAN HOEFNAGEL-HÖHLE UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM.
Conclusions  Results replicate prior reports of effects of font matching on accurate recognition of study items (Reder, et al., 2002)  Higher hits when.
Disrupting face biases in visual attention Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling Introduction Method.
A Comparison of Methods for Estimating the Capacity of Visual Working Memory: Examination of Encoding Limitations Domagoj Švegar & Dražen Domijan
Two systems for reasoning, two systems for learning Harriet Over and Merideth Gattis School of Psychology, Cardiff University.
Simultaneous interpreting is a particularly demanding cognitive process in which language comprehension and production take place at the same time in two.
Elvira Masoura School of Psychology
Example trial sequences for visual perspective-taking task
Rachel L. Fazio, Psy. D. , Allison N. Faris, Psy. D. , Karim Z
Effects of Reading on Word Learning
Alison Burros, Kallie MacKay, Jennifer Hwee, & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien
Parallel or serial, fast or slow, Testing theories of WMC
Ups and Downs Southwest Conference 2007
Emilie Zamarripa & Joseph Latimer| Faculty Mentor: Jarrod Hines
Sami Gülgöz, Fatoş Gökşen, & Çiğdem Kağıtçıbaşı
Review Measure testosterone level in rats; test whether it predicts aggressive behavior. What would make this an experiment? Randomly choose which rats.
Levels of Processing Memory Model (LoP)
Working Memory and Language
Visual Memory is Superior to Auditory Memory
Noticing and Text-Based Chat
Bowden, Shores, & Mathias (2006): Failure to Replicate or Just Failure to Notice. Does Effort Still Account for More Variance in Neuropsychological Test.
Alison Burros, Nathan Herdener, & Mei-Ching Lien
The Effects of Musical Mood and Musical Arousal on Visual Attention
The involvement of visual and verbal representations in a quantitative and a qualitative visual change detection task. Laura Jenkins, and Dr Colin Hamilton.
Oliver Sawi1,2, Hunter Johnson1, Kenneth Paap1;
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
THE NATURE OF SPEAKING Joko Nurkamto UNS Solo.
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
Cognitive Approach Short-term memory – a limited amount of processing takes place here. Short term memory has a very limited capacity (7 items +/- 2)
Cognitive Aspects of CLIL Teaching: Arithmetic in the Classroom Piet Van de Craen, Katrien Mondt & Marielyne Millecam Cognitive Aspects of CLIL | pag.
Marty W. Niewiadomski University of Toronto at Scarborough
Implicit Memory SNU CSE Son, Suil.
Evidence for the Working Memory Model.
Dense Regions of View-Invariant Features Promote Object Recognition
Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology
Teaching Listening Comprehension
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Differences in verb and noun comprehension in aphasia
Episodic retrieval of visually rich items and associations in young and older adults: Evidence from ERPs Kalina Nennstiel & Siri-Maria Kamp Neurocognitive.
Presentation transcript:

1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom Grammatical encoding: a resource-limited process? Robert J. Hartsuiker1, Pashiera N. Barkhuysen2, & Matt Watson3 Introduction Speaking is clearly constrained by cognitive resources - consider for example how difficult it is to simultaneously hold a conversation and maneuver a car through rush hour traffic. But what is the nature of these resource-limitations? According to Levelt (1989) the Conceptualizer is limited by Working Memory, but the Formulator and Articulator ‘are claimed to be largely automatic’ (p. 21). Jou and Harris (1992) supported this claim by showing dual-task effects on conceptualization (e.g., number of pauses) but not on grammatical encoding (e.g., producing number concord). But Just and Carpenter (1992) proposed a different account: There is a single ‘verbal working memory’ (VWM) capacity that constrains each subcomponent of sentence processing. Individuals vary in the size of this capacity, and capacity is taxed in conditions with extrinsic verbal loads. Although Just and Carpenter’s research addressed language comprehension, they claimed that production is similarly constrained by VWM-resources. This study tests whether producing number agreement between subject and verb is constrained by VWM resources. We exploited the phenomenon that sentence fragments with a distributive (multiple token, MT) reading (1) elicit more number agreement errors than items with a nondistributive (Single Token, ST) reading (2) in a task where the fragment is repeated and completed (relative to number-matched controls (1a, 2a). (1) Multiple token (MT): The date on the coins (1a) MT Control: The date on the coin (2) Single Token (ST): The cage with the bears (2a) ST Control: The cage with the bear Distributivity effects were found in several languages. Vigliocco and Hartsuiker (2002) interpreted this as evidence for a ‘’maximal input” hypothesis: the formulator uses all the information it can, not just the strictly necessary information it needs. Logic of the experiments If grammatical encoding is limited by VWM resources, speakers should switch from maximal input to minimal input when resources are scarce, assuming that speakers pay a price for maximal input: having to maintain both the strictly necessary information and the redundant / conflictual information. Experiment 2: Dutch 64 students at Nijmegen University repeated and completed auditory presented sentence fragments (8/condition) using an adjective. 32 participants performed a dual-task: recalling a three-word memory load presented before each adjective. VWM-span was assessed using Daneman & Green’s (1986) speaking span test. Fig. 3 and Fig. 4 show the proportion of agreement errors by load and span (lowest vs highest quartile). Thus, the hypothesis that production of agreement is constrained by VWM predicts that distributivity interacts with VWM-span and with verbal memory load: there should be a larger effect of distributivity if more resources are available. In line with that prediction, Hartsuiker et al. (1999) found that agrammatic patients (hypothesized to suffer from pathological VWM-limitations) showed no distributivity effect, whereas healthy controls did. Experiment 1: English 48 students at Edinburgh University repeated and completed 10 visually presented Multiple Token (1) and 10 Single Token (2) sentence fragments and matched control fragments (1a-2a). Each preamble was preceded by an adjective to be used in the completion. There were two sessions, separated by at least 3 weeks. In one session, a dual-task was performed (recognition of the adjectives). After each session, a memory span task was administered: a series of month names was presented, and participants reported them back in the order of the year (Almor et al., 2001). The percentage of agreement errors for MT and ST items in dual-task and single-task conditions is shown in Figure 1. (There were no errors in the control conditions (1a-2a)). Fig. 3 Agreement errors by load (%) Fig. 4 Agreement errors by span (%) There were significant effects of distributivity, dual-task and speaking span on percentage agreement errors. Distributivity did not interact with load. The (significant) interaction between distributivity and span was in the opposite from predicted direction - there was a smaller distributivity effect for high-span speakers. There was a significant distributivity effect on percentage agreement errors (cf., Eberhard, 1999). The effect of dual task load was not significant, there was no effect of span (median-split, average of two sessions), and no interaction between any factor. Fig. 5 shows dual-task performance (percentage words forgotten). More words were forgotten for items with plural local nouns than with singular local nouns, and more words were forgotten with ST and control items than with MT and control items. These factors did not interact. Fig. 1 Agreement errors by load (%) Fig. 5 Words forgotten (%) Conclusions Manipulations of VWM failed to induce “minimalist processing” (only considering strictly necessary information): Distributivity effects remained, or even increased, in low-capacity conditions. Nevertheless, these experiments clearly show that VWM does a play a role in the production of agreement: manipulations of load, and comparisons of span groups revealed significantly higher error rates when capacity is low (Experiment 2). Furthermore, number conflicts in the sentence fragments interfered with dual-task performance: both speed of recognition and quality of recall were negatively affected by whether the two nouns in the fragment mismatched in number. Figure 2 shows dual-task RTs for correct responses. RTs were slower for ‘No’ responses than for ‘Yes’ responses; slower for items with plural local nouns (1-2) than singular local nouns (1a-2a); and slower for MT and MT control items than for ST and ST control items. These factors did not interact. Fig. 2 dual-task RTs (ms)

Experiment 1: primary and dual task 1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Tilburg, Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom Primary task: repetition and completion of visually presented sentence fragment, using adjective provided, e.g.: … Presented: fast The sailboat on the sea Response: “The sailboat on the sea was fast” Dual task: 1-back recognition task. Another adjective is presented after the response, and the participant decides (by pressing a key) if that was the adjective used in the sentence before this one: Presented: fast The sailboat on the sea Response: “The sailboat on the sea was fast” Presented: OLD Response: <NO-key> Presented: yellow The banana for the monkey Response: “The banana for the monkey was yellow” Presented: FAST Response: <YES-key>

Experiment 2: primary and dual task 1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Tilburg, Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom Primary task: repetition and completion of sentence fragment, using adjective provided, e.g.: Presented: fast The sailboat on the sea Response: “The sailboat on the sea was fast” Dual task: recall of three words, presented prior to the adjective and fragment, e.g.: Presented: attitude labor promise Presented: fast The sailboat on the sea Response: “The sailboat on the sea was fast” Response: “attitude, labor, promise”

Experiment 1: Month sorting task 1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Tilburg, Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom December Series with list lengths 2 - 8 5 lists per series Score is number of lists sorted correctly (max 35) M =21; SD = 3.13 Test-retest reliability: r = .59 (*) 0 sec May 1 sec October 2 sec March 3 sec GO 4 sec “March, May, October, December”

Experiment 2: Speaking span task 1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Tilburg, Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom candles Series with list lengths 2 - 5 5 lists per series Score is number of correct(ly ordered) sentences (max 70) M = 38.5; SD = 5.89 0 sec joy 1 sec talked 2 sec force 3 sec GO 4 sec “The candles are lit, Soccer gives me great joy, We talked for hours, May the force be with you”

References 1Department of Experimental Psychology University of Ghent, Belgium Robert.Hartsuiker@rug.ac.be 2University of Tilburg, Netherlands 3University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom Almor, A., MacDonald, M. C., Kempler, D., Andersen, E. S., & Tyler, L. K. (2001). Comprehension of long distance number agreement in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 35-63. Daneman, M., & Green, I. (1986). Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 1-18. Eberhard, K. M. (1999). The accessibility of conceptual number to processes of subject-verb agreement in English. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 560-578. Hartsuiker, R. J., Kolk, H. H. J., & Huinck, W. J. (1999). Subject-verb agreement construction in agrammatic aphasia: The role of conceptual number. Brain and Language, 69, 119-160. Jou, J., & Harris, R. J. (1992). The effect of divided attention on speech production. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 301-304. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vigliocco, G., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2002). The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 442 - 472.