Stuart A. West, Claire El Mouden, Andy Gardner 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 8: The Evolution of Social Behavior What is social behavior? –Types of social interactions The Conundrum of Altruism Kin Selection or Inclusive.
Advertisements

EVOLUTION & ETHICS.  a social behavior counts as altruistic if it reduces the fitness of the organism performing the behavior, but boosts the fitness.
Behavior, Eusociality, and Kin Selection. OLD: Today: Behaviors Vary.
Evolution of the Family Evolution by Kin Selection Genetic Trait Expressed in Actor (Ego) Must Affect Genotypic Fitness of Individual Related to Actor.
Reciprocal Altruism Elbert Lim Anthro 179. Reciprocal Altruism Term was coined by Robert Trivers (1970’s). Refers to the offering and receiving of support,
Altruism: Voluntary or Coerced? Altruism Requires Decrease Actor’s Direct Fitness Increase Recipient’s Direct Fitness Social Insects Eusociality  Sterile.
Fundamental Concepts in Behavioural Ecology. The relationship between behaviour, ecology, and evolution –Behaviour : The decisive processes by which individuals.
BIOE 109 Summer 2009 Lecture 9- Part II Kin selection.
Types of Social Interactions
EVOLUTIONARY BASES OF BEHAVIOR. DARWIN’S INSIGHTS Wrote On the Origin of Species Identified natural selection as the mechanism that controls the process.
Units of Selection. We think that the only way that adaptations can arise is through natural selection. The effects of such adaptation can be seen at.
Maintaining Cell Identity through Global Control of Genomic Organization Gioacchino Natoli Immunity Volume 33, Issue 1, Pages (July 2010) DOI: /j.immuni
Hamilton’s Rule – Kin Selection. KIN SELECTION & ALTRUISM Kin Selection: selection of a trait through helping relatives, either 1.descendant kin (offspring):
1.Behavior geneticists study the genetic basis of behavior and personality differences among people. 2.The more closely people are biologically related,
CHAPTER 51 BEHAVIORAL BIOLOGY Copyright © 2002 Pearson Education, Inc., publishing as Benjamin Cummings Section D2: Social Behavior and Sociobiology (continued)
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Benjamin Cummings PowerPoint Lectures for Biology, Seventh Edition Neil Campbell and Jane Reece.
Chapter 35 Behavioral Ecology. Define behavior.  Behavior encompasses a wide range of activities.  A behavior is an action carried out by muscles or.
Lana Crosbie A2 EVOLUTIONARY APPROACH SLIDE 1 Approaches to psychology Evolutionary Approach.
1) Relatedness “r” A) means degree of shared genetic similarity among relatives over-and-above the baseline genetic similarity within a population B) ranges.
Kin Selection, Genetic Selection, and Information- dependent strategies By JC Santos, Thomas Valencia, Jannall Brummell.
Behavioral Ecology. Important concepts: Fixed action patterns (FAP’s) Imprinting Kinds of learning: Classical Operant Inclusive fitness and altruism.
Promiscuity and the evolutionary transition to complex societies C. Cornwallis, S. West, K. Davis & A. Griffin Nature; 2010.
CHAPTER 51: Animal Behavior
Where we’ve come so far…
ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCE
Is cooperation viable in mobile organisms
Preferences for visible white sclera in adults, children and autism spectrum disorder children: implications of the cooperative eye hypothesis  Nancy.
Sex-Specific Aging in Flies, Worms, and Missing Great-Granddads
Kinship and Inclusive Fitness
Alturistic Social Behaviors
Cardiorespiratory Fitness and Exercise Training in African Americans
The Matching Hypothesis
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Metabolic Acceleration in Human Evolution
Inclusive Fitness and Its Implications
Chapter 15: How Organisms Evolve.
Theories of Altruism Contrast two theories of altruism.
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
Animal Behaviour Part II
Volume 9, Issue 2, Pages (August 2011)
The Evolution of the Algorithms for Collective Behavior
Sensory Ecology: Night Lights Alter Reproductive Behavior of Blue Tits
Evolutionary Explanations for Cooperation
Module 08: Behavior Unit 2: Individuals and Populations
Evolution and Genes.
Adam Friedman, Norbert Perrimon  Cell 
Wilma Ziebuhr, Jörg Vogel  Molecular Cell 
Adaptation and Inclusive Fitness
Group Formation, Relatedness, and the Evolution of Multicellularity
Circadian Clock Genes Universally Control Key Agricultural Traits
A. Oliver, A. Mena  Clinical Microbiology and Infection 
A Two-Way Street between Attention and Learning
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Animal Social Behavior
Evolution of human cooperation without reciprocity
Volume 49, Issue 2, Pages (January 2013)
Behavioral Ecology (Chapter 53)
Volume 64, Issue 6, Pages (December 2009)
The Normalization Model of Attention
Mark J. Buckley, Natasha Sigala  Neuron 
David Quigley  Journal of Investigative Dermatology 
Quorum sensing and the confusion about diffusion
Inclusive Fitness Vivian Hubby.
Volume 13, Issue 4, Pages (October 2013)
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Navigating the Deubiquitinating Proteome with a CompPASS
Matthew D. Weitzman, Jonathan B. Weitzman  Cell Host & Microbe 
Kin Selection versus Sexual Selection: Why the Ends Do Not Meet
Connecting with an Old Partner in a New Way
Achievement Standard v2
Presentation transcript:

Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans  Stuart A. West, Claire El Mouden, Andy Gardner  Evolution and Human Behavior  Volume 32, Issue 4, Pages 231-262 (July 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Fig. 1 Inclusive fitness is the sum of direct and indirect fitness (Hamilton, 1964). Social behaviours affect the reproductive success of self and others. The impact of the actor's behaviour (yellow hands) on its reproductive success (yellow offspring) is the direct fitness effect. The impact of the actor's behaviour (yellow hands) on the reproductive success of social partners (blue offspring), weighted by the relatedness of the actor to the recipient, is the indirect fitness effect. In particular, inclusive fitness does not include all of the reproductive success of relatives (blue offspring), only that which is due to the behaviour of the actor (yellow hands). Also, inclusive fitness does not include all of the reproductive success of the actor (yellow offspring), only that which is due to its own behaviour (yellow hands; adapted from West et al., 2007a). A key feature of inclusive fitness is that, as defined, it describes the components of reproductive success which an actor can influence, and therefore which they could be appearing to maximise. Evolution and Human Behavior 2011 32, 231-262DOI: (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Fig. 2 A classification of the explanations for cooperation. Direct benefits explain mutually beneficial cooperation, whereas indirect benefits explain altruistic cooperation (Hamilton, 1964). Within these two fundamental categories, the different mechanisms can be classified in various ways (Bergmüller et al., 2007; Frank, 2003; Lehmann & Keller, 2006; Sachs et al., 2004; West et al., 2007a). These possibilities are not mutually exclusive—for example, a single act of cooperation could have both direct and indirect fitness benefits, or interactions with relatives could be maintained by both limited dispersal and kin discrimination. Our dividing up of conditional enforcement strategies is for illustration only, as a detailed discussion is beyond the scope of this paper, and provided elsewhere (Bergmüller et al., 2007) (adapted from West et al., 2007a). Evolution and Human Behavior 2011 32, 231-262DOI: (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Fig. 3 The different types of group selection. The white circles represent cooperators, whereas the grey circles represent relatively selfish individuals who do not cooperate. Panel A shows the “old” group selection, with well-defined groups with little gene flow between them (solid outline). Competition and reproduction is between groups. The groups with more cooperators do better, but selfish individuals can spread within groups. Panel B shows the “new” group selection, with arbitrarily defined groups (dashed lines), and the potential for more gene flow between them. The different groups make different contributions to the same reproductive pool (although there is also the possibility of factors such as limited dispersal leading to more structuring), from which new groups are formed. Panel C shows the “newer” group selection, which emphasises the more proximate mechanism of inter-group competition as a factor shaping the evolution of social behaviours. Panel D shows cultural group selection, in which social behaviours can be horizontally transmitted between group mates, for example with all individuals in the group imitating the behaviour of one “teacher” (T). Evolution and Human Behavior 2011 32, 231-262DOI: (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Fig. 4 The scope of inclusive fitness theory and group adaptation. Irrespective of the extent to which selection is within or between groups, natural selection will lead to organisms that appear to be maximising their inclusive fitness (Frank, 1986; Grafen, 2006a; Hamilton, 1975). In contrast, individuals will only be selected to maximise group fitness in the extreme scenario where there is negligible within group selection (Gardner & Grafen, 2009). Evolution and Human Behavior 2011 32, 231-262DOI: (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions