Laparoscopic Skill Acquisition: The need to adapt to disrupted hand-eye mappings Tina Klein.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
Advertisements

Two types of empirical questions Descriptive –This kind of empirical question requires a researcher to describe some aspect of behavior –For example, a.
Challenges in Surgical Simulators Muhammad Muddassir Malik Muhammad Adil Usman SEECS, NUST, Islamabad School of Electrical Engineering & Computer Science,
Bridget C. Hendricks, James Comerford, Frank Thorn, and Eli Peli The New England College of Optometry, Boston, MA Contrast Matching With Complex and Natural.
Virtual Reality Simulators for Minimal Invasive Surgery Training
Training Generalized Spatial Transformation Skills Giorgio Ganis Harvard University Stephen M. Kosslyn Harvard University Nora S. Newcombe Temple University.
Comparing Video Games and Laparoscopic Simulators in the Development of Laparoscopic Skills in Surgical Residents Barbara J. Adams MD, Franklin Margaron.
A haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled MSc Marcin Morański Professor Andrzej Materka Institute of Electronics,
Matthew Roberts, Vanessa Bacal, Mohammed Mahdi, Ethan D. Grober Mount Sinai & Women’s College Hospital, Division of Urology, Department of Surgery, University.
Methods Inhibition of Return was used as a marker of attention capture.  After attention goes to a location it is inhibited from returning later. Results.
Participants Interference in Motor Learning MEASUREMENTSExperimental ResultsIntroduction  The history of prior action in the human motor system is known.
The effect of varying the visual context on trajectory planning and perceptual awareness of one’s own performance 1 Psychology and Neurocognition Laboratory.
Minimal Invasive Surgery
Spatial Visualization Training Using Interactive Animations Cheryl A. Cohen Mary Hegarty University of California, Santa Barbara Department of Psychology.
Navigation and orientation in 3D user interfaces: the impact of navigation aids and landmarks Author: Avi Parush, Dafna Berman International journal of.
Randomized double-blinded trial investigating the impact of a curriculum focused on error recognition on laparoscopic suturing training J Bingener, T Boyd,
Expertise Differences in Fixation, Quiet Eye Duration, and Surgical Performance During Identification and Dissection of the Recurrent Laryngeal Nerve Harvey,
Chapter 3 Human Resource Development
Simulators for Objective Differentiation of Force-Based Laparoscopic Skills: Towards a Salient Haptic Skills Trainer Dane E. Smith, Joseph Singapogu, Timothy.
The geometry of the system consisting of the hyperbolic mirror and the CCD camera is shown to the right. The points on the mirror surface can be expressed.
Breaking It Down Is Better Haptic Decomposition of Complex Movements Aids in Robot- Assisted Motor Learning J. Klein, S. Spencer, & D. Reinkensmeyer IEEE.
Proficiency-Based Training for Phacoemulsification Princeton Lee Research Fellow RCSI & ICO Alcon Education Night 27/02/2009.
In both active and passive groups, the correlation between spatial ability and performance was attenuated, relative to previous studies In contrast to.
Comparing Performance on the Virtual Laparoscopic and Robotic Simulators Among Medical Students Pursuing Surgical versus Non-surgical Residencies Amanda.
Effect of laterality-specific training on visual learning Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Remote Surgical Robotics Control Systems and Human-Machine Interfacing.
ROBOTIC SURGERY. INTRODUCTION Robotic surgery is an amalgamation of technology and surgical sciences. Robotic surgery is an amalgamation of technology.
It is believed that when inter-trial time is not controlled, the CI effect will occur (i.e., random practice will outperform blocked practice in retention),
Design and Evaluation of a Virtual Reality Simulation Module for Training Advanced Temporal Bone Surgery Sudanthi Wijewickrema Department of Surgery (Otolaryngology)
Designing Scoring Rubrics
The dominant component of pain after gynecologic laparoscopic surgery
Social effects on performance
Conversation Limits Attention: The Impact of Conversation Complexity
Priming of Landmarks During Object-Location Tasks:
Oh, Kenny J. , Esposito, Andrew
An Evaluation of the Effect of Early Childhood Education on the
Variation in Shoulder Elevation
Experiment Basics: Designs
David Marchant, Evelyn Carnegie, Paul Ellison
1 University of Hamburg 2 University of Applied Sciences Heidelberg
Quantitative Evaluation of Surgical Skills Learned in Simulation
UCMAS: A cognition tool
Development of a virtual reality training system for endoscope-assisted submandibular gland removal  Takehiro Miki, Toshinori Iwai, Kazunori Kotani, Jianwu.
Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces
Neurofeedback of beta frequencies:
The Effect of Inter-letter Spacing on Reading Yu-Chi Tai, PhD James E
NATURE NEUROSCIENCE 2007 Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep Daoyun Ji & Matthew A Wilson Department of Brain.
ANTICIPATION.
Table 1. Mean Response Times for Local and Global
From Action Representation to Action Execution: Exploring the Links Between Mental Representation and Movement William Land1,2,3 Dima Volchenkov3 & Thomas.
Eddie Gonzalez & Tyler Muscianes Kin 330L
Adaptive Response to Low Dose Radiation
What is qualitative research?
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Gait, Fluid Cognition, and Balance
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Same - Different Analysis
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Individual differences in field independence influence the ability to determine accurate needle angles  Ahmad Y. Sheikh, MD, Madeleine Keehner, PhD, Audrey.
Neural Mechanisms of Visual Motion Perception in Primates
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Neuroplasticity.
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr
Sensation and Perception
Multisensory Integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony
Stan Van Pelt and W. Pieter Medendorp
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Simulation-driven Enterprise Modelling: WHY ?
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Presentation transcript:

Laparoscopic Skill Acquisition: The need to adapt to disrupted hand-eye mappings Tina Klein

Background: Laparoscopic Surgery Advantages of laparoscopic surgeries over open surgeries: Less risk of infection Faster recovery time Challenges associated with laparoscopic surgeries Long learning curves, caused by the perceptual-motor distortions inherent in the laparoscopic environment Reduced depth information Reduced haptic information Disruption of the hand-eye mapping

Background: Laparoscopic Surgery The motto for medical training has been: “see one, do one, teach one”. This teaching approach is not feasible for laparoscopic skill acquisition. Laparoscopic simulator

Background: Laparoscopic Surgery To keep the disruption of the hand-eye mapping at a minimum, the camera is positioned directly in front of the surgeon (if possible) with a downward view onto the target tissue. However, this camera position is not always possible. Sometimes the camera has to be located to the surgeon’s side, increasing the disruption of hand-eye mapping. The remainder of this presentation will focus on disruptions of hand-eye mapping induced by various camera positions in the laparoscopic training environment.

Background: Basic research on disruptions of hand-eye mapping Cunningham’s (1989) apparatus: Cunningham (1989): Participants made pointing movements with a stylus Participants experienced different degrees of disruptions of hand-eye mapping

Background: Basic research on disruptions of hand-eye mapping Hypothesized perceptual-motor adaptation mechanisms: Gradual adaptation process occurs up to a given threshold 180°: Axis inversion (relatively easy) For rotations between threshold and 180°: axis inversion + gradual backward rotation

Perceptual-motor adaptation versus mental rotation. Results by Cunningham (1989) Results by Shepherd and Metzler, 1971 Adapting to disruptions of hand-eye mappings, like those utilized by Cunningham (1989), are qualitatively different from performing mental rotations.

How about adapting to sideways camera rotations in the laparoscopic environment? Mental rotation versus perceptual motor adaptation Medical journals have previously reported that mental rotation skills correlate with laparoscopic skill acquisition (Hedman et al., 2006). Is performance adaption to disruption of hand-eye mapping in the laparoscopic environment really consistent with mental rotation performance? If performance adaptation to sideways laparoscopic camera rotations requires mental rotation, performance should get worse as the rotation angle increases, with 180° rotations resulting in worst performance. If performance adaptation to sideways laparoscopic camera rotations involves the same mechanisms as outlined by Cunningham (1989), than performance at the 180° should be superior than that observed around 90° and 135° rotations.

Some medical journals stated that performing laparoscopies performance involves mental rotation. Is that really the case? We assessed whether Cunningham’s findings generalize to the laparoscopic training environment (Klein, Wheeler, & Craig, 2015). Used two tasks: Pointing task Peg-transfer task

Results by Klein, Wheeler, Craig (2015) Pointing Task Peg-Transfer Task

Results by Klein, Wheeler, Craig (2015) Are the results consistent with perceptual-motor adaption or with mental rotation? Perceptual-motor adaption

Carry-over effects caused by subsequent camera rotations in the laparoscopic training environment. Hypothesized perceptual-motor adaptation mechanisms: Is performance at the second camera rotation facilitated when both the first and the second rotation use the same perceptual-motor adaptation mechanisms? How is performance impacted when the first and second camera rotation use different perceptual-motor adaptation mechanisms? Gradual adaptation process occurs up to a given threshold 180°: Axis inversion (relatively easy) For rotations between threshold and 180°: axis inversion + gradual backward rotation

Background on carry-over effects Abeele and Bock 2001: Participants performed a computerized joystick tracking task Participants were assigned to one of eight groups. Each group experienced two rotations (initial rotation and a subsequent rotation). Results: 150 Group Initial Rotation Subsequent Rotations   45° Increasing Rotations Group 1 45 90 Group 2 75 120 Group 3 105 150 Group 4 135 180 45° Decreasing Rotations Group 5 Group 6 Group 7 Group 8

Background on carry-over effects Abeele and Bock 2001: Participants performed a computerized joystick tracking task Participants were assigned to one of eight groups. Each group experienced two rotations (initial rotation and a subsequent rotation). Results: Group Initial Rotation Subsequent Rotations   45° Increasing Rotations Group 1 45 90 Group 2 75 120 Group 3 105 150 Group 4 135 180 45° Decreasing Rotations Group 5 Group 6 Group 7 Group 8

What do these results potentially suggest about the presence of thresholds?

One or two thresholds? Increasing condition: No performance improvement was observed when testing at 150° (with prior exposure to the 105° rotation). Decreasing condition: No performance improvement was observed when testing at 75° (with prior exposure to the 120° rotation) Is there one threshold that is the same for increasing or decreasing rotations that is located in the overlapping areas of the two ranges (105 ° - 150 ° and 75° - 120 °)? or Are the thresholds for increasing and decreasing rotations positioned at different rotations?

Follow-up Experiment by Abeele and Bock 2001) Different RMSE peaks for the increasing and the decreasing conditions suggest that the threshold (at which the strategy shift takes place) occurs at different rotation angles for increasing and decreasing rotations.

Do Abeele and Bock’s findings generalize to the laparoscopic environment? Neilson et al., (2017) Participants: 96 undergraduates Apparatus: Laparoscopic training simulator Participants were assigned to one of eight groups and performed a peg-transfer task Group Initial Rotation Subsequent Rotations   45° Increasing Rotations Group 1 45 90 Group 2 75 120 Group 3 105 150 Group 4 135 180 45° Decreasing Rotations Group 5 Group 6 Group 7 Group 8

Do Abeele and Bock’s findings generalize to the laparoscopic environment? Neilson et al., (2017) Participants: 96 undergraduates Apparatus: Laparoscopic training simulator Participants were assigned to one of eight groups and performed a peg-transfer task

Do Abeele and Bock’s findings generalize to the laparoscopic environment? Yes, they did…

Are there different thresholds for increasing and decreasing rotations? Is there one threshold (same for increasing and decreasing rotations, located in the overlapping area of the two ranges). 105 150

Subsidiary experiment Participants were assigned to either the increasing or the decreasing condition. Participants performed one transfer at each rotation Increasing rotation: worst performance at 120° Decreasing rotation: worst performance at 110°. At first sight, it looks like the threshold for the increasing and the decreasing rotations is identical, but is this conclusion consistent with our prior findings?

At first sight, it looks like the threshold for increasing and decreasing rotations is identical (~110°-120°), but is this conclusion consistent with our prior findings? With one threshold at ~110°-120°, these performance improvements should not have occurred. Possibility: The threshold for increasing rotations might be at ~ 120°, while the threshold for decreasing rotations might be between 100° and 105°. However, our subsidiary experiment did not assess performance between 100° and 105°.

Conclusion Adapting to disruptions of hand- eye mappings, like those experienced in the laparoscopic environment, are qualitatively different from performing mental rotations. There might be different thresholds for increasing and decreasing rotations. Future research: Do these findings generalize to expert surgeons?

References Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2001). Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: Different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Experimental Brain Research, 140(4), 407-410. doi:10.1007/s002210100846 Cunningham, H. A. (1989). Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception And Performance, 15(3), 493-506. doi:10.1037/0096- 1523.15.3.493 Hedman, L., Ström, P., P. Andersson, P., Kjellin, A., Wredmark, T., & Felländer-Tsai, L. (2006). High-level visual-spatial ability for novices correlates with performance in a visual-spatial complex surgical simulator task. Surgical Endoscopy, 20, 1275–1280. doi: 10.1007/s00464-005-0036-6 Klein, M. I., Wheeler, N. J., & Craig, C. (2015). Sideways camera rotations of 90° and 135° result in poorer performance of laparoscopic tasks for novices. Human Factors, 57(2), 246-261. doi:10.1177/0018720814553027 Neilson, B. N. & Klein, M. I. (2017). Assessment of performance carry-over effects due to successive exposure to different lateral camera rotations in a laparoscopic training environment. Manuscript submitted for publication. Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701-703. doi:10.1126/science.171.3972.701