Exploring Spatial Frequency Channels in Stereopsis

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Seeing 3D from 2D Images. How to make a 2D image appear as 3D! ► Output and input is typically 2D Images ► Yet we want to show a 3D world! ► How can we.
Advertisements

Psychology 4051 Assessing Vision in Infants and Toddlers.
COMPUTATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE FINAL PROJECT – DEPTH VISION Omri Perez 2013.
PSYC 1000 Lecture 21. Selective Attention: Stroop.
Chapter 8: Vision in three dimensions Basic issue: How do we construct a three-dimension visual experience from two- dimensional visual input? Important.
Binocular Disparity points (C) nearer than fixation (P) have crossed disparity points (F) farther than fixation have uncrossed disparity.
Read Pinker article for Thurs.. Seeing in Stereo.
Careers for Psychology and Neuroscience Majors Oct. 19th5-7pm in SU 300 Ballroom B.
Chapter 5 Human Stereopsis, Fusion, and Stereoscopic Virtual Environments.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 20 Stereo, Motion.
1 Perception, Illusion and VR HNRS 299, Spring 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth.
CS332 Visual Processing Department of Computer Science Wellesley College Binocular Stereo Vision Region-based stereo matching algorithms Properties of.
Evaluating Perceptual Cue Reliabilities Robert Jacobs Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences University of Rochester.
Perception and VR MONT 104S, Fall 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth
Outline Of Today’s Discussion 1.Some Disparities are Not Retinal: Pulfrich Effect 2.Random-Dot Stereograms 3.Binocular Rivalry 4.Motion Parallax.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 16 Stereopsis.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 18 Stereopsis III.
Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 17 Stereopsis II
Signal Detection Analysis of a Three Alternative Forced Choice Signal Detection Analysis of a Three Alternative Forced Choice in a Stereoacuity Context.
Depth Perception, with Emphasis on Stereoscopic Vision
Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2016 Lecture 15 Stereopsis
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Click the Speaker button to listen to Exploring Psychology.
The “Flash-Lag” Effect Occurs in Audition and Cross-Modally
Space Perception and Binocular Vision
Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?
Evidence for Surface-Based Processing of Binocular Disparity
Binocular Stereo Vision
Stereopsis: How the brain sees depth
“What Not” Detectors Help the Brain See in Depth
Space Perception and Binocular Vision
Binocular Stereo Vision
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Perception We have previously examined the sensory processes by which stimuli are encoded. Now we will examine the ultimate purpose of sensory information.
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Detection of Human Simultaneity Thresholds Using Cross-Modal Stimuli
Christopher C. Pack, Richard T. Born, Margaret S. Livingstone  Neuron 
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Measuring Gaze Depth with an Eye Tracker During Stereoscopic Display
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System
Binocular Disparity and the Perception of Depth
Volume 26, Issue 13, Pages (July 2016)
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Syed A. Chowdhury, Gregory C. DeAngelis  Neuron 
Karl R Gegenfurtner, Jochem Rieger  Current Biology 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
The Effects of Spatial Frequency on Stereoacuity: A Replication of Schor and Wood Abby Oostendorp, Michael Palazzolo, Sophie Topouzoglou, Joseph LaBarre,
Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Receptive Fields of Disparity-Tuned Simple Cells in Macaque V1
Binocular Stereo Vision
Vision: Simple stereopsis
MT Neurons Combine Visual Motion with a Smooth Eye Movement Signal to Code Depth-Sign from Motion Parallax  Jacob W. Nadler, Mark Nawrot, Dora E. Angelaki,
Psychology Department, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH
4. Visual Sensory Systems
Albert V. van den Berg, Jaap A. Beintema  Neuron 
4. Visual Sensory Systems
Stereoscopic Surface Perception
PERCEPTION Def: the mental process of organizing sensory input into meaningful patterns.
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Experiment 1 design. Experiment 1 design. A, Differences in cortical spacing in peripheral vision. Top row, Screen coordinates of stimuli in peripheral.
Optimizing Visual Motion Perception during Eye Movements
on Local and Global Stereo Acuity
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Presentation transcript:

Exploring Spatial Frequency Channels in Stereopsis E15JCY15- Psychology Department -Stereoacuity: how good you are at stereopsis -local stereopsis: DOG -global stereopsis: RDS -spatial frequency: light across spatial dimension, graphing amount of light across space -creates black and white bars -Maxwellian view system: controls amount of light entering the pupil and also aligns the subject (using bite bar) -Method of constant stimuli: all possible permutations are presented randomly and data is statistically analyzed after -Define crossed and uncrossed disparity Nicholas C. Duggan, Jonathan Neilio, Olivia C. Rebel, Sarah Meier, Andrew J. Kitt, and Wm Wren Stine University of New Hampshire, Psychology Department, Durham, NH Definitions Method of Limits: A gradual increase or decrease in stimulus level until it can be perceived Sensitivity Threshold: The lowest level of disparity that can be detected. Results Introduction Stereopsis is the perception of depth as a result of receiving visual inputs from two eyes. Humans or other animals that use two visual inputs to create a single perceived image are using binocular vision (McColl, Ziegler, & Hess, 2000). For humans who have normal binocular vision, there is a limit to the smallest possible difference in depth we can perceive. This is known as the depth disparity threshold (Ujike & Ono, 2001). Depth is seen over a wide range of angles, recorded with the unit arc min. Depth disparity can be either crossed or uncrossed. Crossed disparity occurs when the object is in front of the fixation point and uncrossed disparity occurs when the object is behind the fixation point. (Ogle, 1952) Cormack, Stevenson, & Schor (1993) found that there are a large number of disparity channels or narrow band channels. Each channel is corresponds to the size of the element in the image. Some channels are tuned to a higher spatial frequency or small elements, while lower spatial frequency channels are tuned to larger elements. All of the channels account for both crossed and uncrossed disparity. We hypothesize that the narrow band channels are essentially independent because they are tuned to different sized stimuli. If this is true, the narrow band channels should have different disparity thresholds and elements of one spatial frequency should not affect the detection of elements of a different spatial frequency. Our objective for this study is to the map out the architecture of each channel in order to determine the minimal amount of difference in depth that can be detected. Discussion The top two graphs show our anticipated results. A random dot stereogram with a noise mask at the same spatial frequency as the target should be more difficult to detect than one with a non-matching mask. This should result in a matching-mask RDS having a smaller disparity threshold than a non-matching RDS. Yet, the bottom two graphs do not display this trend. One reason for this irregularity may be that the method of limits does not accurately illustrate the effect of a noise mask on disparity threshold. One could also speculate that only having only two subjects running the experiment could possibly produce unusual results. Methods A set of random dot stereograms (RDS) for two spatial frequencies were generated with MATLAB to test a subject’s stereoacuity. Each set of random dot stereograms was displayed with a noise stimulus at a matching spatial frequency or at a non-matching spatial frequency. Subjects observed these random dot stereograms through a Maxwellian view system. Subjects were shown two pairs of random dot stereograms. One would have crossed or uncrossed disparity and the other would have zero disparity. Subjects would press a key to indicate whether the first or second stimulus had disparity. Subjects would receive feedback on the correct answer. If the subject chose correctly, the next stimuli would have a smaller disparity, making depth more difficult to perceive. For an incorrect answer, the opposite would occur. Anaglyphs References Cormack, I. K., Stevenson, S. B., & Schor, C. M. (1993). Disparity-tuned channels of the human visual system. Visual Neuroscience, 10, 585-596. McColl, S. L., Ziegler, L., & Hess, R. F. (2000) Stereodeficient subjects demonstrate non-linear stereopsis. Vision Research 40 1167-1177. Ogle, K. N. (1952). Disparity limits of stereopsis. Archives of Ophthalmology. 48, 50- 60. Ujike. H., Ono, H. (2001). Depth thresholds of motion parallax as a function of head movement velocity. Vision Research 41 2835-2843.