From: Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Chess players' eye movements reveal rapid recognition of complex visual patterns: Evidence from a chess-related visual search task Journal of Vision.
Advertisements

Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;6(4):1. doi: /tvst Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
From: Asymmetric interference between the perception of shape and the perception of surface properties Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):13. doi: /
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Binocular visual acuity for the correction of spherical aberration in polychromatic and monochromatic light Journal of Vision. 2014;14(2):8. doi: /
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(16):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Temporal eye movement strategies during naturalistic viewing
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/5/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Figure Legend: From: Probing intermediate stages of shape processing
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Optimum spatiotemporal receptive fields for vision in dim light
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Three-dimensional motion aftereffects reveal distinct direction-selective mechanisms for binocular processing of motion through depth Journal of.
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Edge detection and texture classification by cuttlefish
From: A double dissociation of the acuity and crowding limits to letter identification, and the promise of improved visual screening Journal of Vision.
From: Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review
Journal of Vision. 2005;5(11):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(5):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Threshold and Variability Properties of Matrix Frequency-Doubling Technology and Standard Automated Perimetry in Glaucoma Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
From: Suppression of monocular visual direction under fused binocular stimulation: Evoked potential measurements Journal of Vision. 2005;5(1):4. doi: /5.1.4.
Presentation transcript:

From: Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review Journal of Vision. 2011;11(5):13. doi:10.1167/11.5.13 Figure Legend: Spatial summation for the detection of a homogeneous spot of light in central and peripheral vision. (a) Schematic illustration of Riccò's and Piper's law of spatial summation. (b) Spatial summation in peripheral view for two observers (monocular, 15° nasal, dark adapted, 12.8 ms). Data by Graham and Bartlett (1939, Table 2). (c) Diameter of receptive and perceptive fields for the human, monkey, and cat. Open squares: Human perceptive fields, mean of temporal and nasal data provided by Oehler (1985, Figure 4). Open circles: Monkey perceptive fields, obtained by using the Westheimer paradigm (Oehler, 1985, Figure 8). Filled circles: Monkey receptive field (De Monasterio & Gouras, 1975, Figure 16, broad-band cells). Crosses and filled triangles: receptive fields of the cat (Fischer & May, 1970, Figure 2). Analyses by Strasburger (2003b), figures modified from Strasburger (2003). Date of download: 10/19/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.