Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 5: Space and Form Form & Pattern Perception: Humans are second to none in processing visual form and pattern information. Our ability to see patterns.
Advertisements

Can You Match These Friends? A Test of Genetic Similarity Theory Katrina M. Sandager, Stephanie R. A. Maves, Sarah L. Hubert, and April Bleske-Rechek University.
Comparison of Spatial and Temporal Discrimination Performance across Various Difficulty Levels J.E. THROPP, J.L. SZALMA, & P.A. HANCOCK Department of Psychology.
A Colour Face Image Database for Benchmarking of Automatic Face Detection Algorithms Prag Sharma, Richard B. Reilly UCD DSP Research Group This work is.
I. Face Perception II. Visual Imagery. Is Face Recognition Special? Arguments have been made for both functional and neuroanatomical specialization for.
Knowledge Representation: Images and Propositions Chapter 7.
CONFIDENCE – ACCURACY RELATIONS IN STUDENT PERFORMANCES We attempted to determine students’ ability to assess comprehension of course material. Students.
F UTURE R ESEARCH Q UESTIONS Will our short form suffice when comparing females to males? Will an examination of the learning curve across items provide.
Sectional Views Section Views.
Pilot: Customizing a Commercially Available Digital Game to Assess Cognitive Function William C. M. Grenhart, John F. Sprufera, Jason C. Allaire, & Anne.
Introduction Neuropsychological Symptoms Scale The Neuropsychological Symptoms Scale (NSS; Dean, 2010) was designed for use in the clinical interview to.
Individual Preferences for Uncertainty: An Ironically Pleasurable Stimulus Bankert, M., VanNess, K., Hord, E., Pena, S., Keith, V., Urecki, C., & Buchholz,
Spatial Visualization: Is There a Gender Difference? Terri Bennett Southern CT State University January 16, 2010.
Dissociation of Egocentric and Object-Centric Processing in Mental Rotation Tao, W.D. 1, Yan, J.J. 1, Wang, P. 1, Zhou, L. 1, and Sun, H-J. 2 1 Southwest.
FMRI and Behavioral Studies of Human Face Perception Ronnie Bryan Vision Lab
Adults’ Responsiveness to Infant Facial Expressions Chinmay Aradhye and Dr. Jennifer Vonk Dept. of Psychology, Oakland University Methods Part 1 1)Forty-five.
Poster presented at APS 2014 Abstract This study was conducted to determine if explaining criminal behavior influences later identification. Schooler and.
Lifespan Development Physical and Cognitive Development in Middle Childhood Chapter 9.
It is believed that when inter-trial time is not controlled, the CI effect will occur (i.e., random practice will outperform blocked practice in retention),
Stimuli were presented on a 17 inch monitor (in a dimly lit room), operating at 60 Hz with a resolution of 1280 x Two objects of the same type (teapot.
Body Position Influences Maintenance of Objects in Visual Short-Term Memory Mia J. Branson, Joshua D. Cosman, and Shaun P. Vecera Department of Psychology,
Identifications By: Tyler Hansberry. One of the biggest problems police may face with an investigation is the need to confirm or recognize an unknown.
Visual Estimation of Three- and Four-body Center of Mass Jay Friedenberg Psychology Department Manhattan College Jay Friedenberg Psychology Department.
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Alison Burros, Kallie MacKay, Jennifer Hwee, & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien
Figure 4: Sample House Drawings
Attachment style and condom use across and within dating relationships
Recognizing Visual and Auditory Stimuli
Personality Assessment, Measurement, and Research Design
African-American Stimuli
Feature Binding: Not Quite So Pre-attentive Erin Buchanan and M
Effects of Color and Emotional Arousal on Visual Perception
The Effects of Imagery on Name Recall
Unit 4: Perceptual Organization and Interpretation
SPECULAR FLOW AND THE PERCEPTION OF SURFACE REFLECTANCE
CEN3722 Human Computer Interaction Cognition and Perception
Stephen P. Joy, Ph.D. Emili Dubar, B.A. Albertus Magnus College
Colour Discrimination Task
Kinematics Introduction to Motion
Annina Brendel - Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University
Visual Memory is Superior to Auditory Memory
Sectional Views Section Views.
Mastering Autodesk Revit MEP 2016 CHAPTER 21: Creating Devices
Stephen P. Joy, Ph.D. Albi Beshi, B.A. Heather Mills, B.A.
Alison Burros, Nathan Herdener, & Mei-Ching Lien
Which of these is “a boy”?
NUMERACY AND MENTAL COMPUTATION WITH ITS PERSPECTIVE ON BRAIN HEMISPHERICITY ASYMMETRY: WHAT’S RIGHT AND WHAT’S LEFT. TSG 6 Ilda Salwani Ameer and Parmjit.
The involvement of visual and verbal representations in a quantitative and a qualitative visual change detection task. Laura Jenkins, and Dr Colin Hamilton.
Creative Meta-seeing: Constructive Visual Thinking
Recognition Memory of Visual, Audio & Written Dialogue in Film
Inverse Transformation Scale Experimental Power Graphing
Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
How you perceive your surroundings
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Visual Processing in Fingerprint Experts and Novices
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Perception.
Personality Assessment, Measurement, and Research Design
A Channel for 3D Environmental Shape in Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex
Same - Different Analysis
Karl R Gegenfurtner, Jochem Rieger  Current Biology 
Serial Dependence in the Perception of Faces
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Introduction to Perception: Visual Perception
Sectional Views Section Views.
Perception.
Judging Peripheral Change: Attentional and Stimulus-Driven Effects
What do we see? #Perceiving images
What do we see? #Perceiving images
Presentation transcript:

Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces Jennifer Zhu Stephen P. Joy, Ph.D. Amity Regional High School Albertus Magnus College Results Abstract As shown in Figure 2, accuracy was greatest when the target face was shown in full-face (the original orientation). Accuracy declined in a linear fashion the greater the angle of rotation. When the target appeared in profile (90o rotated), accuracy was reduced to an approximately chance level. As shown in Figure 3, reaction time was most rapid when the target was presented in full face and increased linearly with the angle of rotation – with the exception that at 90o of rotation, when facial recognition was at chance levels, participants responded more rapidly than at 60o of rotation. With only three males in the sample to date, we cannot test meaningfully for gender effects. However, the overall accuracy of the males (mean = .51, SD = .11) did not differ appreciably from that of the females (mean = .49, SD = .08). Nor did reaction times appear to differ by gender. We studied mental rotation of human faces to see whether performance conformed to established findings using abstract shapes. Sets of faces (1 target, 2 foils) were presented in varying rotations under each target; 350 trials were completed per participant. Accuracy diminished, and reaction time increased, with increasing rotation. Introduction Mental rotation tasks involve identifying objects presented from different angles. Previous research has used abstract shapes (constructed of blocks) as stimuli and consistently found that greater degrees of rotation produce lower accuracy and longer reaction times; people actually rotate the mental image until they find (or fail to find) a match. Another consistent finding has been a male advantage on this task. Maeda & Yoon (2013) report a mean effect size of .57 (Hedges’ g). This is a typical stimulus set:   A few studies have used other types of stimuli, but no previous study has examined whether these principles hold for human faces. The closest approach involves rotating faces away from the vertical (Valentine & Bruce, 1988) or studies of the facial inversion effect, in which some faces are presented upside-down (Rakover & Teucher, 1997). Recognizing people from different angles is an essential social skill. However, since identification of human faces involves specific neural pathways (distinct from those used for analyzing spatial figures), it cannot be assumed that both tasks are performed similarly. Furthermore, the gender effect found with such shapes may not generalize to faces. Any modest male superiority on spatial tasks could well be offset by a modest female superiority in perceiving socially important cues. Method We created realistic human faces using FaceGenTM. Five male and 5 female target faces were generated. For each target, 2 foils were created; these were 30% randomized. This degree of relationship is comparable to that between siblings; the intent was for the foils to resemble, but be distinguishable from, the targets. In each trial, one target was presented in the upper portion of the screen, with the target and two foils in the lower portion. The three faces in the lower half could be presented in any combination of 7 orientations: full-face or 30, 60, or 90 degrees rotated to left or right. These rotations were randomized, as were the positions of the correct options. Trials were presented in blocks of 5, male and female blocks alternating. Altogether, 350 slides (35 male and 35 female blocks) were created. SuperLabTM software was used to present the stimuli and measure accuracy and reaction times.   Participants (N = 21 to date, including 18 females) were college undergraduates who received extra credit. We are aware of the need to add more participants, but our results (with the exception of gender analyses) are clear enough to present at this time. Figure 1 shows two of the slides: one male set and one female set. In the first case, the correct response is the face on the right, seen in full-face. In the second case, the correct response is the middle face, see in profile. Figure 1 Sample Stimuli STICIM Min Max M SD Interdependence 2.29 6.00 4.57 0.93 Sexual Intimacy 3.40 4.93 0.95 Comfort Intimacy 1.43 5.86 4.16 1.23 Trust 3.25 4.53 0.92 Warmth Motivation 2.50 1.00 Sexual Desire 3.20 5.21 0.91 Trust Motivation 3.75 5.18 0.64 IC Overall 3.08 5.84 4.55 0.77 IM Overall 3.70 4.99 0.70 Graphic Indicators M SD Colors 4.70 2.34 Warm Colors 2.45 1.59 DYR Overall 19.33 8.49 Discussion Not surprisingly, these findings suggest that people use a similar mental rotation mechanism when processing human faces to that used when processing abstract shapes. It also seems likely that the male advantage on mental rotation may be absent when human faces are used as stimuli, but this will require additional data. Such a finding would make evolutionary sense and would call in question the idea that males always display superiority on mental rotation tasks. The consistent male advantage identified to date may be an artifact of the type of stimuli used. Aside from the currently inadequate sample size, the major limitation of the present study was the fact that the faces turned out to be more difficult to distinguish than originally intended – so much so that accuracy diminished to chance levels beyond a certain degree of rotation. Future variations on this experiment should use faces with varying degrees of relatedness. It will also be interesting to test the effects of rotation along other axes and the possibility of interaction effects when more than one axis is involved. References Maeda, Y., & Yoon, S.Y. (2013). A meta-analysis on gender differences in mental rotation ability measured by the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test: Visualization of Rotations. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 69-94. Rakover, S.S., & Teucher, B. (1997). Facial inversion effects: Parts and whole relationship. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 752-761. Shepard, R.N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701-703.  Valentine, T., & Bruce, V. (1988). Mental rotation of faces. Memory & Cognition, 16, 556-566. For further information contact: sjoy@albertus.edu