Experimental Evidence for Strategic Use of Humor Styles in Relationship Initiation Theresa E. DiDonato1 and Brittany K. Jakubiak2 Loyola University Maryland,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Schulte-Hostedde, A.I., Eys, M.A., & Johnson,K. (2008). Femalemate choice is influenced by male sport participation. Evolutionary Psychology, 6,
Advertisements

Cross Cultural Research
An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship Desirability Within Hook-up Culture: Nice-Guys vs. Bad-Boys An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship.
Relationships Psychology.
The Dating Game: The Importance of Female Laughter as a Receptivity Signal ANTHONY R. GAROVE & SALLY D. FARLEY.
Study 1: Method Demographics 346 men and 299 women 41.1% Dating, 18.2% Cohabiting, 4.2% Engaged or 18.9 Married Mean age of sample was 31.7 years Procedure.
Genetic Factors Predisposing to Homosexuality May Increase Mating Success in Heterosexuals Written by Zietsch et. al By Michael Berman and Lindsay Tooley.
The Person in the Situation: Self-Concept, Gender, and Dispositions
The role of humor in human relationships: An evolutionary model
Romantic Red: Red Enhances Men’s Attraction to Women Andrew J. Elliot and Daniela Niesta University of Rochester Elliot, A., & Niesta, D. (2008 November).
Origins of Attraction MATTHEW CORRINET. Biological: Fischer et al. (2003)  “... used an fMRI... to investigate blood flow in the brains of 20 men and.
Biosocial Approach Gender Development. Gender Characteristics, whether biological or socially influenced, by which people define male and female WomenMen.
By: Deanna Duermit, Mikayla Mowzoon, Jenna Tioseco
Quantitative Research Statistics are like a bikini. What they reveal is suggestive, but what they conceal is vital Aaron Levenstein.
Sex differences in romantic kissing among college students: An evolutionary perspective Summary by Amber Kika, Nina Dangourian, and Esmeralda Huerta For.
ATTRACTION 1. 2 INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION The desire to approach other people.
Reactions to Humorous Comments and Implicit Theories of Humor Styles Nicholas A. Kuiper, Gillian A. Kirsh, and Catherine Leite Presented by Willie Hallford.
Template provided by: “posters4research.com”   Ideals: mental constructs that represent an idea of traits we are attracted to in potential partners (Fletcher.
Women Control Male Romantic Partners to Pursue Extra Pair Partners INTRODUCTION MATE GUARDING AND MATE RETENTION Mate guarding controls with whom the female.
Mankind’s Greatest Blessing: Personal Humor Style and Humor Appreciation By: Steven LaCorte The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between.
Charismatic Stories as a Basis for Leader Development Logan M. Steele University of Oklahoma Logan L. Watts University of Oklahoma Michael D. Mumford University.
University of Texas at El Paso
IB Psych 10/05/16 Today’s Agenda: Results of IQ “Test”
California State University, Long Beach
Attachment style and condom use across and within dating relationships
©2013 McGraw-Hill Companies
The Relationship Between Instagram Photo Editing and Undergraduate College Women’s Body Dissatisfaction Madeline Wick, Cindy Miller-Perrin, & Jennifer.
Sexiness on Social Media Hurts Men Too
Sex Differences in Gender, Orientation, and Identity
Rhonda N. Balzarini, MA University of Western Ontario E:
Moral Foundations Predict Adult Mating Desire
Chapter 5: Genes, Culture, and Gender
Sexual Imagery & Thinking About Sex
13 Leadership.
Cognitive APPROCH TO EATING DISRODER
Unit 3: Biological Psychology
Christian Hahn, M.Sc. & Lorne Campbell, PhD
Aggression Types as Predictors of Adolescent Substance Use
SELF-DOUBT EFFECTS DEPEND ON BELIEFS ABOUT ABILITY
Are masculine males attractive
Evolution & Sexual Selection
Influence of Meeting Humor Styles on Meeting Satisfaction
STEM Communal Affordances
SAINT LOUIS UNIVERSITY
The Matching Hypothesis
Observer Participants
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Developing Leadership Potential with Stories of Pragmatic Leaders
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Evidence for gender bias in interpreting online professor ratings
Sociosexuality and Perceptions of Partner Over Time
Relationships Parental Investment.
Psychological origins of attraction
Natural Sampling versus Mental Concepts Whitney Joseph
Starter Imagine - you did not do as well as you wanted to in a biology test, but your teacher praises you for working hard and trying your best. You feel.
Sexual Dimorphism Male Male Female Female Male Female
Mate Preferences Evolutionary lens model.
2University of Virginia
What You Need to Know.
Baumeister & Tice Chapter 2
PS4029/30 Perspectives on social attributions
Biological influences on gender
Parental Investment and Sex Differences in Sexual Behavior
Relationships Lesson 1.
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
A Social Learning Approach Martina Miller
Effects of Sexualization in Advertisements
Presentation transcript:

Experimental Evidence for Strategic Use of Humor Styles in Relationship Initiation Theresa E. DiDonato1 and Brittany K. Jakubiak2 Loyola University Maryland, Carnegie Mellon University Abstract Do people prefer to use specific humor styles as a function of specific romantic goals? The current experimental study investigated the effect of romantic motives (short-term or long-term) on individuals’ anticipated use of, and response to, positive humor and negative humor. We primed 224 heterosexual participants with a short-term or long-term relationship motive and asked them to report on their intended humor use in an imagined context with an attractive stranger (target). Participants also indicated how their interest would change in response to the imaginary target’s use of positive or negative humor. The evidence suggests that individuals are strategic producers of humor as a function of their romantic goals, avoiding negative humor when pursuing a long-term partnership. Target’s positive humor increased attraction, especially for women. Findings extend a trait-indicator model of humor. Do people use different humor styles as a function of relational goals? Methods Participants 224 single, heterosexual participants (167 female; Mage = 27.12, SD = 12.20) were recruited from an undergraduate participant pool and via Mturk to complete the study, posted on Qualtrics. Design 2 (humor style: positive or negative) x 2 (relational motive: short-term or long-term) x 2 (gender: male or female) mixed factor experimental design to investigate the effect of romantic motives on anticipated humor use, and responses to others’ humor use. Manipulation We manipulated romantic motive using Griskevicius and colleagues’ (2006) mating primes, modified to reflect an opportunity for a short- or long-term relationship rather than having this relationship in order to maintain a relationship initiation focus. Participants were randomly assigned to condition. Measures Anticipated humor production (based off of Martin et al.’s [2003] HSQ): Participants used a scale of 1-7 to indicate the extent to which they would engage in each of 16 humor behaviors in order to secure a relationship consistent with their primed motive (i.e., short- or long-term). Anticipated use was computed as averages separately for positive and negative humor styles. Humor’s Effectiveness: Participants imagined the target in their relationship scenario producing 8 different examples of humor (4 positive style, 4 negative style) and indicated how each would change their romantic interest in the target on a scale of -5 (decrease) to +5 (increase). Averages were computed for positive and negative styles. Figure 1. The anticipated production of positive and negative humor as a function of primed relational motive and gender. Results from a 2 (humor style: positive or negative) x 2 (romantic motive: short-term or long-term) x 2 (gender: male or female) mixed ANOVA showed the expected interaction between humor style and romantic motive on anticipated humor production, F(1, 220) = 8.37, p = .004, η2 =.04. Negative humor use was endorsed more by individuals pursuing short-term relationships (M = 3.61, SD = -.93) than long-term relationships (M = 3.32, SD = 0.99), F(1, 220) = 4.23, p = .041, η2 =.02. There was no difference in the preferred use of positive humor for individuals pursuing short-term (M = 4.67, SD = 0.91) and long-term relationships (M = 4.71, SD = 0.78), F(1, 220) = 0.49, p = .483. Contrary to expectations, participant gender did not independently predict anticipated humor use, nor did it interact with humor style or romantic motive to predict intended humor production (all F’s < 1.10). Theoretical Background People fall in love with funny people. A wealth of evidence supports the idea that humor is desired and attractive (e.g., Lippa, 2007), yet little is known about how humor use might change as a function of romantic goals. Evolutionary theory suggests that humor’s wide appeal in romantic contexts may be because humor is a sexually-selected behavior that serves as an honest (i.e., hard-to-fake) signal of desirable underlying traits, such as intelligence and creativity (Miller, 2000) or favorable good partner/co-parent traits, such as warmth or prosociality (Hall, 2015). However, evidence showing that humorous individuals have these desired traits is mixed. Perhaps the divergent findings reflect a focus on general humor, rather than specific humor styles, which may convey different qualities. Humor can be divided into two primary styles, positive (e.g., affiliative and self-enhancing) and negative (e.g., aggressive and self-defeating; Martin et al., 2003). Consistent with a trait-indicator model, people discriminate between humor styles when evaluating potential long-term partners, reporting more interest in prospects who use positive humor than negative humor, an effect accounted for by warmth inferences (DiDonato, Bedminster, & Machel, 2013). For short-term relationships, however, humor style does not appear to affect romantic interest. In short-term relationships, individuals may prioritize the good genes (e.g., intelligence) conveyed by any kind of funny humor, rather than seeking the good partner traits conveyed specifically by positive humor. Given the link between humor style and relationship interest, suitors may manipulate their humor style use as a function of their motive to secure either a short-term or a long-term relationship. Men may be especially likely to manipulate their humor, as women tend to be humor evaluators (Wilbur & Caombell, 2011). Primary Hypotheses Men and women (especially men) anticipate using positive humor in the pursuit of either long-term or short-term relationships. Men and women (especially men) anticipate using negative humor more in the pursuit of short-term than long-term relationships. For women (the humor evaluators), positive humor will increase short-term and long-term interest; negative humor will only increase interest in short-term contexts. Excerpt Long-term Prompt (female) There you are, sitting outside at your favorite coffee shop. It’s a pleasant early spring day, and you can smell the blooming flowers in the breeze. You have a book open, but you’re not really reading it. You look around, relaxed and daydreaming. As you watch the people strolling by in front of you, you notice that everyone seems to be in a particularly good mood. From behind you, you hear a voice say: “Wow, isn’t this an absolutely beautiful day?” … Excerpt Short-term Prompt (female) Imagine that you are on vacation with your friends on a tropical island. It’s the last day of your trip and you are sitting on the beach on a pleasant summer afternoon, sipping an exotic drink. The air is warm and pleasant, and you watch the waves as the sun begins to set. You have a book open, but you’re not really reading it. Instead, you look around, relaxed and daydreaming. As you watch the people strolling by on the soft sand, you notice that everyone seems to be in a particularly good mood. From behind you, you hear a voice say: “Wow, isn’t that the most beautiful sunset you have ever seen?”… Does romantic interest change as a function of target humor style? Figure 2. Changes in romantic interest (humor’s effectiveness) as a function of humor style, relational motive, and gender. Results from a 2 (humor style: positive or negative) x 2 (romantic motive: short-term or long-term) x 2 (gender: male or female) mixed ANOVA showed that positive humor (M = 3.17, SD = 1.19) increased participants’ interest more than negative humor (M = 1.35; SD = 1.54), F(1, 220) = 212.67, p < .001, η2 =.49. Note that negative humor was still judged to increase attraction, t(223) = 71.34, p < .001, relative to the mid-point (no change) of the participants’ response scale, for individuals primed to imagine long-term, t(116) = 8.27, p < .001, or a short-term relationship, t(106) = 10.50, p < .001. Positive humor increased women’s interest (M = 3.29, SD = 1.11) more than men’s (M = 2.82, SD = 1.35), F(1, 220) = 6.53, p = .012, η2 =.03. Results Descriptive Statistics Manipulation Check Discussion Humor use as a flirtation strategy involves more than simply being funny: the style of humor chosen during relationship initiation appears linked to romantic motive. People opt to use positive to attract someone for a short- or long-term relationship, but negative humor is preferred more for short-term, relative to long-term, contexts. Such strategic use calls into question the validity of humor as an honest signal of underlying traits; however, even strategic use of certain styles of humor may require a certain degree of baseline possession of those traits. In other words, knowledge of a suitor’s motives may provide important clarification on the extent to which a specific humor style is a reliable indicator of underlying personality traits. On the receiving end, positive humor appears to generate more romantic interest, regardless of the receivers’ relational motive, but negative humor also increased romantic interest. Perhaps humor in general is appealing because of its capacity to signal good genes (e.g., intelligence) regardless of style, with positive styles all-the-more attractive because of the good partner traits that might be inferred (e.g., warmth). The results of two manipulation check questions (“To what extent are you interested in a ___ term relationship with this person?”) suggested an effective manipulation of romantic motive. References DiDonato, T. E., Bedminster, M. C., & Machel, J. J. (2013). My funny valentine: How humor styles affect romantic interest. Personal Relationships, 20, 374-390. Griskevicius, V., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Peacocks, Picasso, and parental investment: The effects of romantic motives on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 63-76. Hall, J. A. (2015). Sexual selection and humor in courtship: A case for warmth and extroversion. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 1-15. Lippa, R. A. (2007). The preferred traits of mates in a cross-national study of heterosexual and homosexual men and women: An examination of biological and cultural influences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 193-208 Martin, R. A., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J., & Weir, K. (2003). Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 48-75. Wilbur, C. J., & Campbell, L. (2011). Humor in romantic contexts: Do men participate and women evaluate? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 918-929.