Chapter 11 Opener Both genes and environment contribute to the development of foraging behavior in the honey bee
Figure 11.1 Development of worker behavior in honey bees
Figure 11.1 Development of worker behavior in honey bees (Part 1)
Figure 11.1 Development of worker behavior in honey bees (Part 2)
Figure 11.2 Gene activity varies in the brains of nurse bees and foragers.
Figure 11.3 Social environment and task specialization by worker honey bees
Figure 11.4 Levels of the messenger RNA produced when the for gene is expressed in the brains of nurses and of foragers in three typical honey bee colonies
Figure 11.5 Imprinting in greylag geese
Figure 11.6 Cross-fostering has different imprinting effects in two related songbirds
Figure 11.6 Cross-fostering has different imprinting effects in two related songbirds (Part 1)
Figure 11.6 Cross-fostering has different imprinting effects in two related songbirds (Part 2)
Figure 11.7 Spatial learning by chickadees
Figure 11.7 Spatial learning by chickadees (Part 1)
Figure 11.7 Spatial learning by chickadees (Part 2)
Figure 11.8 A Clark’s nutcracker holding a seed in its bill that the bird is about to cache underground
Figure 11.9 Kin discrimination in Belding’s ground squirrels
Figure 11.9 Kin discrimination in Belding’s ground squirrels (Part 1)
Figure 11.9 Kin discrimination in Belding’s ground squirrels (Part 2)
Figure 11.10 Belding’s ground squirrels learn their own odor
Figure 11.11 Migratory routes taken by blackcap warblers in the fall
Figure 11.11 Migratory routes taken by blackcap warblers in the fall (Part 1)
Figure 11.11 Migratory routes taken by blackcap warblers in the fall (Part 2)
Figure 11.12 Differences in the migratory behavior of two closely related birds, the black redstart and the common redstart
Figure 11.13 A coastal Californian garter snake about to consume a banana slug, a favorite food of snakes in this region
Figure 11.14 Response of newborn, naive garter snakes to slug cubes
Figure 11.15 A tongue-flicking newborn garter snake senses odors from a cotton swab that has been dipped in slug extract
Figure 11.16 Genetic differences cause behavioral differences in fruit fly larvae
Figure 11.17 A single genetic difference has a large effect on maternal behavior
Figure 11.18 Social amnesia is related to the loss of a single gene
Figure 11.18 Social amnesia is related to the loss of a single gene (Part 1)
Figure 11.18 Social amnesia is related to the loss of a single gene (Part 2)
Figure 11.19 Surrogate mothers used in social deprivation experiments
Figure 11.20 Socially isolated rhesus infants that are permitted to interact with one another for short periods each day at first cling to each other during the contact period
Figure 11.21 Developmental homeostasis in humans
Figure 11.22 Developmental switch mechanisms can produce polyphenisms within the same species
Figure 11.23 Tiger salamanders occur in two forms
Figure 11.24 Activity of the gene that codes for gonadotropin-releasing hormone in the cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni
Figure 11.25 Subordinate males of the fish Astatotilapia burtoni react very quickly to the absence of a dominant rival
Figure 11.25 Subordinate males of the fish Astatotilapia burtoni react very quickly to the absence of a dominant rival (Part 1)
Figure 11.25 Subordinate males of the fish Astatotilapia burtoni react very quickly to the absence of a dominant rival (Part 2)
Figure 11.26 Developmental flexibility in redback spiders
Figure 11.26 Developmental flexibility in redback spiders (Part 1)
Figure 11.26 Developmental flexibility in redback spiders (Part 2)
Figure 11.26 Developmental flexibility in redback spiders (Part 3)
Figure 11.26 Developmental flexibility in redback spiders (Part 4)
Figure 11.27 Male thynnine wasps can learn to avoid being deceived by an orchid
Figure 11.27 Male thynnine wasps can learn to avoid being deceived by an orchid (Part 1)
Figure 11.27 Male thynnine wasps can learn to avoid being deceived by an orchid (Part 2)
Figure 11.28 Spatial learning abilities differ among members of the crow family
Figure 11.29 Male pinyon jays make fewer errors than females do when retrieving seeds from caches they have made, especially after intervals of 2 to 4 months
Figure 11.29 Male pinyon jays make fewer errors than females do when retrieving seeds from caches they have made, especially after intervals of 2 to 4 months (Part 1)
Figure 11.29 Male pinyon jays make fewer errors than females do when retrieving seeds from caches they have made, especially after intervals of 2 to 4 months (Part 2)
Figure 11.30 Sex differences in spatial learning ability are linked to home range size
Figure 11.31 A virtual maze used for computer-based studies of navigational skills
Figure 11.32 Sex differences in the hippocampus
Figure 11.33 Operant conditioning exhibited by a rat in a Skinner box
Figure 11.34 Biases in taste aversion learning
Figure 11.35 Vampire bats could not form learned taste aversions
Figure 11.35 Vampire bats could not form learned taste aversions (Part 1)
Figure 11.35 Vampire bats could not form learned taste aversions (Part 2)