From: Does print size matter for reading

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):27. doi: /
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 10/25/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Bayesian inference for psychometric functions
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Subjectively homogeneous noise over written text as a tool to investigate the perceptual mechanisms involved in reading Journal of Vision. 2013;13(11):14.
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(16):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Design of a Wireless Biological Signal Conditioning System1
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Does print size matter for reading From: Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography Journal of Vision. 2011;11(5):8. doi:10.1167/11.5.8 Figure Legend: Distribution of roman text sizes (x-heights) in books and type specimens from the 15th, 16th, and 18th centuries. Fifteenth century Italian types cluster at the large end of the range, while 16th century French types span nearly the whole range, and 18th century English and French types extend the small end of the range. Date of download: 10/27/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.