Journal of Vision. 2014;14(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Chess players' eye movements reveal rapid recognition of complex visual patterns: Evidence from a chess-related visual search task Journal of Vision.
Advertisements

From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
From: Davida Teller Award Lecture 2013: The importance of prediction and anticipation in the control of smooth pursuit eye movements Journal of Vision.
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Focus information is used to interpret binocular images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccades
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(10):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9.
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Nonlinearities in color coding: Compensating color appearance for the eye's spectral sensitivity Journal of Vision. 2006;6(9):12. doi: /
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Multisensory simultaneity judgment and proximity to the body
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(6):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/16/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi: /3.8.2.
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Optimizing wavefront-guided corrections for highly aberrated eyes in the presence of registration uncertainty Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):8. doi: /
From: The Effect of Spectacle Lenses Containing Peripheral Defocus on Refractive Error and Horizontal Eye Shape in the Guinea Pig Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The psychometric function: The lapse rate revisited
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;6(4):5. doi: /tvst Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images
Presentation transcript:

From: Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving Journal of Vision. 2014;14(12):21. doi:10.1167/14.12.21 Figure Legend: Geometrical relationships between the TP, optic flow, and path geometry. (A) A constant curvature path in a constant curvature bend (no steering change required). The FP is on an equal flow cylinder. The TP is on another equal flow cylinder (zero horizontal flow; visual flow is vertical). Multiple TPs (where line of sight is tangential to a curve following the road curvature) are present on the zero flow cylinder (see Figure 1B). Inset A1: Optic flow during linear translation; a focus of expansion specifies the current heading. Inset A2: Optic flow during rotation; direction and magnitude of flow specify direction and rate of rotation. Inset A3: More complex flow is generated by simultaneous translation and rotation (of locomotor axis, the reference axis for the visual field). Here, equal flow cylinders emerge (green, red, blue), with points on the FP (blue) having equal (negative) flow and TPs (red) having zero flow. Note that this is a property of the visual flow field and is not dependent on demarcated lane edges around the path, nor is the TP on the lane edge unique. Eye movements will determine how this pattern appears in retinal flow. Inset A4: In a visual flow representation (see Figure 1B), waypoints on an equal (negative) flow cylinder have the same horizontal component as do TPs on the zero flow cylinder. (B) Curve radius decreases. Constant radius path (no steering change) would result in lane departure on the inside edge (oversteering). TP lies on a constant flow cylinder with inward flow. (C) Curve radius increases. Constant radius path (no steering change) would result in lane departure on the outside edge (understeering). TP lies on a constant flow cylinder with outward flow. Note that in B and C the movement of the TP is in the direction of flow, which is opposite to steering error (i.e., the direction of flow at TP indicates direction of the required correction). Date of download: 10/27/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.