Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2008;8(8):1. doi: /8.8.1 Figure Legend:
From: The Müller-Lyer Illusion: Investigation of a Center of Gravity Effect on the Amplitudes of Saccades Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):11. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(3):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/1/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: Illumination discrimination in real and simulated scenes
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):39. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Pattern matching is assessed in retinotopic coordinates
From: Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response Journal of Vision. 2010;10(2):12. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(5):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion Journal of Vision.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Shape, motion, and optical cues to stiffness of elastic objects
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: Cause and effect
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion Journal of Vision. 2014;14(5):1. doi:10.1167/14.5.1 Figure Legend: Cartoon illustration of the moving-barber-pole display. (a) A realistic scenario in which a moving truck carries three active barber poles. The truck and the carrier gratings inside each pole move independently. In this cartoon illustration, the truck moves horizontally to the left (the modulator) and the gratings within the barber poles (the carrier) translate diagonally up to the right. Therefore, motion of the bar terminators or end-stop motion is up to the left. Although the stimuli in the experiments are generated as the product of a modulator sinewave grating and a carrier sinewave grating viewed through a Gaussian window, they appear very much as the barber poles on moving truck would appear. (b) Remarkably, a stimulus identical to (a) can be produced by moving a snapshot of the barber poles in a particular direction, the direction of rigid translation, which depends on the speed and direction of the modulator and of the carrier. Date of download: 10/28/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.