From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Figure Legend: From: Human grasp point selection
Advertisements

From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):10. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Physiologic statokinetic dissociation is eliminated by equating static and kinetic perimetry testing procedures Journal of Vision. 2016;16(14):5.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
Figure Legend: From: A short-term memory of multi-stable perception
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: statistical inference
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9. doi:10.1167/16.1.9 Figure Legend: The setup for Experiment 3 that tests whether flow discrepancies arising at the borders rather than within the moving object can influence heading perception. Under certain circumstances, one such discrepancy creates radial-like motion contrast between the object and the background at the trailing edge of the moving object (pseudo FoE). (a) In the Object Condition, there is motion contrast between the trailing edge of the object (red arrowheads) and the background (blue arrowheads). In the example depicted, the object occludes the background FoE and the pseudo FoE effect is strong because of the radial-like motion around the trailing edge of the object. (b) In the Blank Object condition, a gap is introduced between the trailing edge of the object and the background (black) to eliminate the motion contrast. The blank Object traveled with the object during the trial. (c) The trajectory of the object was rigidly displaced horizontally in eight different locations relative to the observer's future path (lateral offset). When the lateral offset was zero, the trailing edge made tangential contact with the future path at the end of the trial. Positive and negative lateral offsets correspond to rightward or leftward shifts in the trajectory. Date of download: 10/30/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.