Volume 14, Issue 17, Pages (September 2004)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Refraction at plane surface and Prisms Dr. M K Raghavendra BASE, Bangalore.
Advertisements

Created by Stephanie Ingle Kingwood High School
Moza M. Al-Rabban Professor of Physics
Chapter 23 Mirrors and Lenses.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 32 Light: Reflection and Refraction.
Geometric Optics Conceptual MC Questions. If the image distance is positive, the image formed is a (A) real image. (B) virtual image.
Chapter 23 Mirrors and Lenses.
Reflection vs. Refraction When light strikes a mirror, it is reflected. The law of reflection tells us that the reflected angle is equal to the incident.
The Refraction of Light:. Just what is refraction of light?
1© Manhattan Press (H.K.) Ltd. Reflection Refraction Refraction 12.1 Reflection and refraction Total internal reflection Total internal reflection.
Example: An object 3 cm high is placed 20 cm from (a) a convex and (b) a concave spherical mirror, each of 10 cm focal length. Determine the position.
Ch23 Geometric Optics Reflection & Refraction of Light.
Reflection vs. Refraction
The Refraction of Light: Lenses and Optical Instruments
1 Thin Lens Light refracts on the interface of two media, following Snell’s law of refraction: Light bends through a triangular prism: θ 1 and θ 2 are.
© Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company Preview Objectives Refraction of Light The Law of Refraction Sample Problem Chapter 14 Section 1 Refraction.
Copyright © by Holt, Rinehart and Winston. All rights reserved. ResourcesChapter menu Refraction Chapter 14 Table of Contents Section 1 Refraction Section.
Physics 102: Lecture 17, Slide 1 Physics 102: Lecture 17 Reflection and Refraction of Light.
Chapter 14 Preview Objectives Refraction of Light
Refraction and Lenses. Refraction is the bending of light as it moves from one medium to a medium with a different optical density. This bending occurs.
PHY 102: Lecture Index of Refraction 10.2 Total Internal Reflection 10.3 Prism and Rainbows 10.4 Lenses 10.5 Formation of Images 10.6 Lens Equations.
Millions of light rays reflect from objects and enter our eyes – that ’ s how we see them! When we study the formation of images, we will isolate just.
Homework: Page 525, #2-9. Phenomena Related to Refraction Apparent Depth: A pencil partly under water looks bent when viewed from above Our brain perceives.
Physics Chapter 15: Refraction.
Reflection & Mirrors Topic 13.3 (3 part lesson).
Notes 23.1: Optics and Reflection
Refraction and Lenses.
Index of Refraction.
Chapter 23: Reflection and Refraction of Light
The more rays that reach a point the brighter the point
Chapter 32Light: Reflection and Refraction
Friday, March 25th, 2011 The Law of Reflection.
Chapter 24 Geometric Optics
Chapter 17 Reflection and Refraction
A. WAVE OPTICS B. GEOMETRIC OPTICS Light Rays
Refraction Chapter 14: Section 1.
Refraction and Lenses AP Physics B.
Geometric Optics Ray Model assume light travels in straight line
Phys102 Lecture 21/22 Light: Reflection and Refraction
Rays, Mirrors, Lenses, and Prisms
Chapter 12 Review SNC 2D1 Mr. Dvorsky May
A reminder of the important laws of refraction
Unit 7 Light and Vision.
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Lenses.
Free-Response-Questions
Eye position predicts what number you have in mind
32 Optical Images image formation reflection & refraction
Millions of light rays reflect from objects and enter our eyes – that’s how we see them! When we study the formation of images, we will isolate just a.
Refraction and Lenses Physics.
Reflection Refractive index Snell's law Optical power Lens equation
Millions of light rays reflect from objects and enter our eyes – that’s how we see them! When we study the formation of images, we will isolate just a.
Millions of light rays reflect from objects and enter our eyes – that’s how we see them! When we study the formation of images, we will isolate just a.
A Unique Advantage for Giant Eyes in Giant Squid
Text Reference: Chapter 32.1 through 32.2
Mirrors, Plane and Spherical Spherical Refracting Surfaces
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
Refraction and Lenses Honors Physics.
Stefan Schuster, Roland Strauss, Karl G Götz  Current Biology 
Refraction and Lenses AP Physics B.
OBJECTIVE QUESTIONS FOR NEET AIIMS JIPMER
Stability of Cortical Responses and the Statistics of Natural Scenes
Refraction, Lenses, & Color
Refraction n =1 n =1.3 Bending of light rays at boundaries with different indices of refraction (n) n =1.5 n =2.4.
Fruit-Catching Fish Tune Their Fast Starts to Compensate for Drift
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Refraction and Lenses.
Refraction n =1 n =1.3 Bending of light rays at boundaries with different indices of refraction (n) n =1.5 n =2.4.
Reflection and Refraction of Light
Volume 14, Issue 17, Pages (September 2004)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 14, Issue 17, Pages 1565-1568 (September 2004) Archer Fish Learn to Compensate for Complex Optical Distortions to Determine the Absolute Size of Their Aerial Prey  Stefan Schuster, Samuel Rossel, Annette Schmidtmann, Ilonka Jäger, Julia Poralla  Current Biology  Volume 14, Issue 17, Pages 1565-1568 (September 2004) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050

Figure 1 Experimental Arrangement An archer fish fires at one out of eight differently sized disks, presented behind a glass plate at one out of four heights above the water surface. Current Biology 2004 14, 1565-1568DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050)

Figure 2 The Corrections a Size Constant Visual System Must Make to Account for Light Refraction (A) An illustration of the virtual image of a horizontal disk (at height h above the water surface) as it would appear to a size constant visual system that is unable to account for the optical effects of the water-air interface is shown. The uncorrected apparent image has a larger-than-actual horizontal size (dxv) and also extends into the vertical direction (dyv). To quantitatively derive this a computer program calculates the virtual image point P′ for each point P of the real disk; it applies Snell's law [9] to the light rays within a bundle emanating from P and entering the eye's pupil (center at E, pupil radius δ) to do so. (B) This panel shows how the apparent horizontal size of a disk of 10 mm diameter depends on height h and horizontal distance. Calculations are as illustrated in (A) for the experimentally relevant target heights 200, 400, 600, and 800 mm and for the horizontal distances (horizontal distance between E and P in [A]) that were assumed by the responding fish (see text). The impressive viewpoint dependency of the required corrections can equally well be demonstrated in terms of corrections to apparent distances and angles subtended by the images rather than sizes (results not shown). Current Biology 2004 14, 1565-1568DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050)

Figure 3 Naive Fish Do Have Size Preferences but Do Not Select Objects of the Same Absolute Size (A) An example of results from a fish sized 15 cm is shown. The size of the preferred object increased about 2-fold as height h was increased from 200 to 800 mm (total of n = 162 tests). (B and C) Experiments with two smaller fish are shown at a higher resolution. In these experiments disk sizes differed only by 2 mm (instead of the normal 4 mm). The choices of each fish are shown for two of the four heights (121 tests are shown). Current Biology 2004 14, 1565-1568DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050)

Figure 4 Trained Fish Can Acquire Size Constancy with Remarkable Precision This is an example of tests with a fish (same as in Figure 3A) that had been trained to select an object of 6 mm absolute size at all heights h (200 to 800 mm) despite the viewpoint-dependent distortions in apparent size illustrated in Figure 2 (n = 270). Current Biology 2004 14, 1565-1568DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050)

Figure 5 Archer Fish Form a Concept of Absolute Size Rather Than Interpolating Templates This figure shows experiments we conducted with one of two fish to determine whether the fish had simply stored previously rewarded combinations of apparent image, viewing angle, and height (“templates”) and based its choices on an interpolation among these templates. After having succesfully been trained to recognize a disk sized 6 mm (illustrated as a gray disk in the panels), the fish were subsequently retrained to select a novel target size (10 mm, illustrated as a larger black disk). However, only a partial retraining was given, at 200 and 400 mm. In subsequent tests carried out at all four heights, the fish chose the novel size at the two greater heights as well. This contradicts the template interpolation hypothesis that predicted the choice of the 6 mm disk (gray bar). Current Biology 2004 14, 1565-1568DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.050)