Colour electrochromism and Thomas Bangert thomas.bangert@qmul.ac.uk http://www.eecs.qmul.ac.uk/~tb300/pub/ColourVision+EcoPix.pptx
part 1: The Colour Model The Munsell Colour Model actual mapping to human vision A colour catalog vs a colour model
color catalog vs colour model catalog requires selection of colours based on perceptual matching partial colour model codes spectrum as systematic mixing of wavelengths true colour model codes the color of spectrum X=100,Y=100,Z=0 Yellow
Colour as information a theory of information processing.
Colour Reproduction true colour code + specs of viewer = image colour defined by code viewer can be group or individual display decides how to create colour from code gives perceptual predictability Yellow Orange + bluish-red Magenta
The Standard Observer from colour matching studies CIE1931 xy chromaticity diagram primaries at: 435.8nm, 546.1nm, 700nm The XYZ sensor response Y is defined as luminance difference from Y is the colour information The Math: … 2-d as z is redundant
Understanding CIE chromaticity x and y show difference from Y Best understood as a failed colour circle White in center Saturated / monochromatic wavelengths on the periphery Everything in between is a mix of white and the colour Circular colour models are the holy grail of colour theory … so far no one has succeeded!
But does the CIE model work? Does it match? Problem #1: ‘negative primaries’ Problem #2: no definition of colour
Colour Sensor response to monochromatic light Human Bird 4 sensors Equidistant on spectrum What are these sensors used for? What information is needed? my answer is: Wavelength
How to calculate wavelength with 2 poor quality luminance sensors. 1 . a shift of Δ from a known reference point . 8 G R . 6 . 4 . 2 . λ-Δ λ λ+Δ Wavelength Roughly speaking:
the ideal light stimulus Monochromacy: The reason we see rainbows is because the human visual system works with single wavelength light -- monochromatic light monochromatic stimulus This is the underlying paradigm! Allows wavelength to be measured in relation to reference.
Problem: natural light is not ideal Light stimulus might not activate reference sensor fully. Light stimulus might not be fully monochromatic. ie. there might be white mixed in
Solution: Then reference sensor can be normalized A 3rd sensor is used to measure equiluminance. Which is subtracted. Equiluminance & Normalization – essential to finding wavelength, can also called saturation and lightness
a 4 sensor design 2 opponent pairs only 1 of each pair can be active min sensor is equiluminance
Human Retina only has 3 sensors! What to do? We add an emulation layer. Hardware has 3 physical sensors but emulates 4 sensors No maths … just a diagram!
Testing Colour Opponent model What we should see What we do see There is Red in our Blue – the problem of Purple
Pigment Absorption Data of human cone sensors Red > Green
Dual Opponency with Circularity an ideal model using 2 sensor pairs
a circular colour model We divide colour coding and colour reproduction: Coding no need to link to specific observer – ideal observer not linked specifically to human vision Display decides how best to present colour to observer – making colour anomalies fit
Part 2 – Reproducing Colour Part 1 – Coding Colour fully circular universal ideal observer Part 2 – Reproducing Colour takes knowledge about observer and optimizes/distorts to the individual/group improved or natural reproduction modes
Coding Natural Colour Problem #1: Real world is not monochromatic Spectrum of a common yellow flower
Colour coding … for dual channel opponency Problem # 1 easy to solve we simply assume monochromacy when stimuli are not monochromatic opponent channels simply subtract to 0 green, yellow and red are active r-g = 0 leaving only yellow b = 0 stimuli equivalent to monochromatic
Opponent Coding Only primaries are true colours all other colours are intermediary … and can be generated by proportions of primaries!
Accurate colour reproduction … for humans Problem # 2 Any colour may be displayed by a combination of 2 primaries but the location of primaries can vary between individuals and intermediary locations can be distorted
Accurate colour reproduction … tuned to the individual primaries must be mapped for the individual mid-points must be mapped Provides an individual colour profile … a map of the primaries and intermediary points. 467 517 573 644 545 503 603
tunable primaries 573 644 517 467 W a v e l e n g t h ( n m ) 1 . 2 Yellow 644 1 . 517 . 8 467 Red . 6 Green . 4 Blue . 2 . - .2 3 5 4 4 5 5 55 60 65 700 W a v e l e n g t h ( n m )
Part 3 testing the theory is it sound? is it useful? does human vision use it? Is there empirical evidence to support paradigm + theory? note: a theoretical model about information is the information itself!
Apparatus monochromator light source equal light across visible spectrum
the stimuli
Transition Colour Matching generate subject selectable monochromatic stimuli subject selects colour perceptual primaries are calculated
Results no leading questions -- only “blue” 4 primaries (pure colours) naturally resolve to blue, green, yellow and red primaries are equidistant transitions worked for all subjects Most subjects see peripheral colours red in blue 40% could see “magenta” – blue in red potential problem: people treat purple as if it were primary some colour blind people can’t see purple
histogram of results
results from mapping colour vision
Application natural colour reproduction
Luminance: High Dynamic Range Current display technology: 0.1 – 100 cd/m2 (currently pushed up to 500, but designed for 100 cd/m2) DICOM GSDF: 0.05 – 4,000 cd/m2 (defined for grayscale medical imaging only) Natural environment: 0.01 – 10,000 cd/m2
Coding HDR … using an absolute lumiance code rather than a relative code HDR is here now … using multiple exposure!
the colour of infra-red (650-750nm) not the stereo-type but true infra-red – high wavelength light remove the filter from a digital camera & it will work in the infra-red Images in the infra-red produced by enthusiasts now! What is the colour when you go beyond red?
related work: Dolby
Examples of real world colour? Colours are often computed, not measured!
… an extreme example What is the colour?
Part 4 – Building Real Colour Displays Colour coded based on ideal model colour model based on perception of colour not retinal sensors Colour display tuned to the individual
Electrochromism similar to rechargeable battery electrolyte is source of ions electrical potential pushes ions into electrochromic layer ions cause oxidation/reduction and the absorption/colour changes reverse the polarity and the ions are forced out and the reaction reverses colour change is caused by some wavelengths being absorbed the basic application is a smart window
EcoPix The aim of EcoPix is to turn electrochromism into useful display technology €1,000,000 EU funded project main partners are METU in Turkey, CENTI in Portugal and QMUL full colour (subtractive RGB) displays are intended to be exterior, billboard size with natural illumination
work so far Samples we are receiving from METU: PEDOT:PSS Blue/Cyan single layer devices using ITO as electrodes roughly 35x35mm
Blue/Cyan without white balance / xenon light source light from xenon short – similar to sunlight samples absorb about 50% of light coloured state absorbs further 50% of light
Blue/Cyan Transmittance (white balanced) potential progressively applied
Transparent / Coloured States
Transparent / Coloured state transition frame rate: 25 fps state change takes about 2 frames 80ms very low current order of 100µa for state change in current samples stable for approx. 30s
Lab Facilities Dedicated Colour Lab to deliver QMUL share of EcoPix. Standard 35mm photographic slide projection 50x50mm slides xenon light (used in cinema) room size front and rear projection screen Precision programmable power supply
measurement by rear projection Sensor
Questions? http://www.eecs.qmul.ac.uk/~tb300/pub/ColourVision+EcoPix.pptx http://www.ecopix.eu http://1drv.ms/1uSoffE References Poynton, C. A. (1995). “Poynton’s Color FAQ”, electronic preprint. http://www.poynton.com/notes/colour_and_gamma/ColorFAQ.html Bangert, Thomas (2008). “TriangleVision: A Toy Visual System”, ICANN 2008. Goldsmith, Timothy H. (July 2006). “What birds see”. Scientific American: 69–75. Neitz, Jay; Neitz, Maureen. (August 2008). “Colour Vision: The Wonder of Hue”. Current Biology 18(16): R700-r702. http://blog.dolby.com/2013/12/tv-bright-enough/ http://www.photonics.com/Article.aspx?AID=30712