From: Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response Journal of Vision. 2010;10(2):12. doi:10.1167/10.2.12.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Advertisements

From: Early interference of context congruence on object processing in rapid visual categorization of natural scenes Journal of Vision. 2008;8(13):11.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
Figure Legend: in visual cortex
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(3):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(11):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Vector subtraction using visual and extraretinal motion signals: A new look at efference copy and corollary discharge theories Journal of Vision.
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Estimating predictive stimulus features from psychophysical data: The decision image technique applied to human faces Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):22.
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Suppression of monocular visual direction under fused binocular stimulation: Evoked potential measurements Journal of Vision. 2005;5(1):4. doi: /5.1.4.
Design of a Wireless Biological Signal Conditioning System1
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response Journal of Vision. 2010;10(2):12. doi:10.1167/10.2.12 Figure Legend: Illustration of the technique used to measure phase delays between stimuli seen by the left and right eyes. Flickering lights are presented separately to the left and right eyes ( Input signals), which generate neural signals in response to the flicker ( Intermediate signals). These signals are transmitted to the cortex, where the neural signals from the two eyes are combined ( Output signals). In the top sequence, the left and right eyes are equally light-adapted, so that there should be little phase delay between the signals from the two eyes. As a result, signals that are in opposite phase at the input (left) should destructively interfere at the output, where they produce a flicker null (right). In the middle and bottom sequences, the left eye is more light-adapted than the right eye, so that the signal in the left eye is phase advanced by Δ θ relative to the signal in the right eye. In this case, left and right eye signals in opposition at the input (middle sequence) will no longer null at the output. To restore the null (bottom sequence), the signal seen by the left eye must be phase delayed by Δ θ to compensate for the internal phase advance. Date of download: 11/6/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.