The Neurobiology of Cultural Intelligence

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Gesture and speech are controlled by the same system Maurizio Gentilucci Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Italy RTM (Rete Multidisciplinare.
Advertisements

Figure Three-dimensional reconstruction of the left hemisphere of the human brain showing increased activity in ventrolateral area 45 during verbal.
Chapter 12 The Stages of Learning
Giacomo Rizzolatti and Corrado Sinigaglia. Basic knowledge Mirror mechanism Unifies perception and action Its functional role depends on its anatomical.
NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE SEP 01
Chapter 15: Santos, L. R., Flombaum, J. I., & Phillips, W. The evolution of human mind-reading: How nonhuman primates can inform social cognitive neuroscience.
Mirror Neurons.
From Perception to Action And what’s in between?.
A Unifying View of the Basis of Social Cognition by: Vittorio Gallese, Christian Keysers, and Giacomo Rizzolatti Amanda Issa Angela Arreola Stacy Struhs.
Mind, Brain & Behavior Wednesday February 5, 2003.
Motor cortical areas: the homunculus The motor system.
Motor cortical areas: the homunculus The motor system.
Cortical motor structures. Hierarchical Organization of Motor System.
Recent Findings in the Neurobiology & Neuropsychology of Reading Processes A. Maerlender, Ph.D. Clinical School Services & Learning Disorders.
Psych 216: Movement Attention. What is attention? Covert and overt selection appear to recruit the same areas of the brain.
Evolution of Language Tashina Nadjiwon.
Prediction in Human Presented by: Rezvan Kianifar January 2009.
Comparative Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) Study of Tool Use Pathways in Humans, Apes and Monkeys Ashwin G. Ramayya 1,2, Matthew F. Glasser 1, David A.
Testing computational models of dopamine and noradrenaline dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio.
Background The physiology of the cerebral cortex is organized in hierarchical manner. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) constitutes the highest level of the.
Evolution of Control-Related Mental Models Crystal A. Brandon.
Visual Perception, Attention & Action. Anthony J Greene2.
Cortex for Newbies. Neocortex Gyri (plural: singular = gyrus) – convolution or bump – protruding rounded surfaces (folds) Sulci (plural: singular = sulcus)
Coricelli and Nagel (2008) Introduction Methods Results Conclusion.
Hemispheric Specialization
Cognitive Science Overview Cognitive Science Defined The Brain Assumptions of Cognitive Science Cognitive Information Processing Cognitive Science and.
September 2, 2009 Kamini Krishnan Tandra Toon. Article Focus Review of literature that combines use of functional or structural MRI and microelectrode.
By Prabh, Zoya, Rose, Georgia, Lucy and Humayra. neuron+system&docid= &mi d=8D04BBC9F9C0FAD6037C8D04BBC9F9C0FAD.
Chapter 2. From Complex Networks to Intelligent Systems in Creating Brain-like Systems, Sendhoff et al. Course: Robots Learning from Humans Baek, Da Som.
Articulatory Net I.2 Oct 14, 2015 – DAY 21
Lars Taxén – Activity Modalities An action perspective based on innate coordination capacities Lars Taxén, Linköping University
Our Newer Brain “What makes us distinctively human mostly arises from the complex function of the cerebral cortex.” - David G. Myers.
By: Angela D. Friederici Presented By: Karol Krzywon.
Zatorre paper Presented by MaryKate Chester
Cortical Control of Movement
Discuss two effects of the environment on physiological processes. (22) Discuss (22) – A considered and balanced review, including a range of arguments,
Shared Intentionality
The Cerebral Cortex.
A Unified Coding Strategy for Processing Faces and Voices
Cerebral responses to vocal attractiveness and auditory
Primary motor cortex Domina Petric, MD.
CORTICAL MECHANISMS OF VISION
Who is That? Brain Networks and Mechanisms for Identifying Individuals
Volume 56, Issue 6, Pages (December 2007)
The Cortical Control of Movement Revisited
Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning
The Cortical Motor System
Presented by: Rezvan Kianifar January 2009
Friedemann Pulvermüller  Trends in Cognitive Sciences 
Liping Wang, Lynn Uhrig, Bechir Jarraya, Stanislas Dehaene 
Volume 80, Issue 3, Pages (October 2013)
Neural Correlates of Visual Working Memory
The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update
Negative BOLD Differentiates Visual Imagery and Perception
Distributed Neural Systems for the Generation of Visual Images
Moral Judgments Recruit Domain-General Valuation Mechanisms to Integrate Representations of Probability and Magnitude  Amitai Shenhav, Joshua D. Greene 
Negative BOLD Differentiates Visual Imagery and Perception
The Three Stream Hypothesis
Volume 38, Issue 3, Pages (May 2003)
Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning
Neural and Computational Mechanisms of Action Processing: Interaction between Visual and Motor Representations  Martin A. Giese, Giacomo Rizzolatti  Neuron 
Memory: Dissociating multiple memory processes
Clinical Concepts Emerging from fMRI Functional Connectomics
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (August 2000)
Conserved Sequence Processing in Primate Frontal Cortex
Definition of the NCC. Content-specific NCC (red) directly contribute to phenomenal distinctions (e.g., low-level visual features, faces, or places) within.
César F. Lima, Saloni Krishnan, Sophie K. Scott 
Economic Choice as an Untangling of Options into Actions
Associational cortex introduction
Farshad A. Mansouri, Tobias Egner, Mark J. Buckley 
Presentation transcript:

The Neurobiology of Cultural Intelligence Mosaic of Mirror Neurons. The Neurobiology of Cultural Intelligence Antonella Tramacere Lichtenberg-Kolleg/The Göttingen Institute for Advanced Study  &  The German Primate Center/Leibniz Institute for Primate Research  Georg-August-Universität Göttingen The evolution of cultural intelligence The distinctive aspects of primate cognition evolved mainly in response to the challenging demands of a complex social life constituted by constant competition and cooperation in social groups (social intelligence hypothesis). If primates are social, humans are “ultra-social”. Indeed, whereas primates have evolved social-cognitive skills for competing and cooperating with conspecifics, humans have also evolved skills (i.e., specialized social learning, language and “theory of mind”) that enable them to create different cultural groups (cultural intelligence hypothesis) (Hermann et al. 2007). The social intelligence hypothesis is supported by positive correlations between relative brain size (i.e., neocortex) and social variables, such as group or grooming clique size, as index of social complexity (Dunbar 2003). However, this only provides generic support for the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Overall correlations do not explicate the brain size differences in relation to social cognitive skills, nor do they help us to identify which neural substrates humans deploy during specific cognitive tasks that others primates do not. Neural correlates of social cognition Assuming that mirror neurons (MNs) are an important feature of the social brain (by underlying the capacity of the individual to transform social sensorial information into a motor format), I test whether MNs properties and variations across different primate species confirm the prediction of the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Accordingly, I propose a parsimonious categorization that may help to clarify the properties of MNs by taking into account two unambiguous physiological criteria: the modalities of sensory input triggering the response, and the effectors involved in the motor output. I obtained three main categories: hand (visuomotor) MNs, mouth (visuomotor) MNs and audio-vocal MNs (Tramacere et al. 2016). Mouth Mirror Neurons Mouth MNs (indirectly investigated in humans and macaques) activate during the observation and execution of same or similar actions performed with the mouth. Mouth MNs are likely present from birth, and associated with phenomena of early facial communication in the context of mother – infant interactions. They seem to be a common heritage of anthropoid primates, which have evolved early facial responsiveness and visual attraction to others faces. Elettrophysiological investigations in lemurs, which are supposed to lack these features in the neonatal phase of development, could be instrumental to confirm this hypothesis (Tramacere & Ferrari 2016). A B C Fig. 1. A) Macaque newborn showing attention and responsiveness to others’ mouth actions during lab experimental condition. B) Macaque mother-infant facial exchange. C) Lemur mother carrying infants on the back. Hand Mirror Neurons Hand MNs (directly investigated in marmoset and macaque, and indirectly in chimpanzee and human) activate during observation and execution of manual actions, in correspondent cerebral areas (dorsal stream). However, only in humans hands MNs strongly activate during mimicking actions (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia 2010 ). In contrast, monkey hand MNs are very weakly sensitive to non-goal directed movements (Nelissen et al. 2011 ), while in chimpanzee these neurons activate more than in the monkey, but significantly less strongly than in humans. Further, in macaques there is a large discrepancy between the ventral and the dorsal streams (circuits associated with respectively the coding of the end-result of observed actions, and with the spatial coding of movements and finer level of action kinematics). This difference is less pronounced in chimpanzees and absent in humans (Hecht et al. 2012 ). Fig. 2. Hand mirror neurons (MNs) in humans, chimpanzees and macaques. Specific anatomical regions of human (A), chimpanzee (B) and macaque (C) brains activated during observation of grasping overlap with regions activated during the execution of the same grasping action. Premotor, parietal and sensory areas are shown in green, red and yellow, respectively. The blue arrow represent the dorsal stream. (PM) premotor cortex, (IFG) inferior frontal gyrus, (IPL) inferior parietal lobule, (M1) primary motor cortex, (SMA) supplementary motor area, (STS) superior temporal sulcus, cytoarchitectonic areas BA44 (known as Broca’s area), BA45 and BA6, PF, PFG, FCBm, F5. Audio-Vocal Mirror Neurons Audio-vocal MNs activate during both listening and execution of specific vocal cues. They seem to be associated with auditory feedback and vocal learning (Mooney 2014) in songbirds and in human beings, where they have been investigated respectively directly and indirectly. Whereas humans and songbirds convergently evolved audio-vocal MNs, non-human primates lack the neural substrates for vocal learning. Indeed, while the emergence of a rudimentary auditory-motor control in monkey brains under conditional learning shares many features with the neural pathways of auditory-vocal integration that has been observed in expert vocal learners, monkey vocal conditioning seems to not be associated with any audio-vocal MNs (Coude´ et al. 2011 )—suggesting that it relies on different mechanisms from human speech. Fig. 3. Songbirds and humans, both vocal learners, have a similar pattern in the way premotor/motor areas are connected to the brainstem in the establishment of learned vocalizations. Both non-songbirds and macaques (which lack these patterns) can be trained to vocalize voluntarily and consequently to establish connections between the premotor cortex and motoneurons. However, no evidences exist in relation to the possibility to develop audio-vocal MNs in premotor or regions of the brain. (RA) robust nucleus of the arcopallium, (XII) motoneurons of the syrinx, (HVC) high vocal centre, (Area X) striatal nucleus, (DLM) dorsolateral nucleus of the medial thalamus, (LMC) face area of the primary motor cortex, (Am) nucleus ambiguous, (Ast) anterior striatum, (At) anterior thalamus (PAG), periacqueductal grey area, (ACC) anterior cingulate cortex. A role for Mirror Neurons in the Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis The comparative analysis of various sub-types of MNs can be a useful tool for identifying potential mechanisms underlying the evolution of social cognition, and testing the predictions of the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Specifically, considering MNs as a mosaic of distinct although interrelated traits can be instrumental to identify evolutionary changes that occur in some brain parts, without simultaneous changes in other parts. Mechanisms of neural mirroring seem to be evolved in different primate species with different timing and precursors, with a relatively independent evolutionary history and in relation to different social and environmental demands. Further, humans seem to be endowed with more sophisticated and multiple mirroring mechanisms. Indeed, if mouth MNs may not be implicated in unique humans social capacities, hand and audio-vocal MNs seem to associated with action-processes oriented social and vocal learning (Tramacere and Moore 2016).