Brain Connectivity Lab

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
Advertisements

Learning, Volatility and the ACC Tim Behrens FMRIB + Psychology, University of Oxford FIL - UCL.
Both self-esteem and co-rumination have been shown to influence an individual’s psychological well-being. Rose (2002) defined co- rumination as “excessively.
Phonetic Similarity Effects in Masked Priming Marja-Liisa Mailend 1, Edwin Maas 1, & Kenneth I. Forster 2 1 Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing.
Journal Club Alcohol and Health: Current Evidence July-August 2006.
Participants: 21 smokers (13M, ages 18-45) and 21 age-, gender-, race-, and education-matched controls. Procedure: Stimuli were 100 photographs: 50 food.
Distinguishing Evidence Accumulation from Response Bias in Categorical Decision-Making Vincent P. Ferrera 1,2, Jack Grinband 1,2, Quan Xiao 1,2, Joy Hirsch.
TREATING SPECIAL POPULATIONS. OVERVIEW Tobacco Treatment Smoking Outcomes Co-occurring Disorders Integration Tobacco Prevention.
Nicotine Holley Williamson and Rachel Martinez. Addiction Chewed, Smoked, or Inhaled Absorbed within 7 Seconds of inhalation Nicotine molecule shaped.
Study 1: 23 Ss viewed 265 pictures of food, attractive faces, symbols indicating monetary gains, and neutral objects. Stimuli were rated on 14 dimensions.
Motivational Interviewing (MI) is effective and efficient for fostering health behavior change, and the elicitation of “change talk” (CT: i.e., any expression.
Testing computational models of dopamine and noradrenaline dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio.
SMOKING in ADOLESCENTS with PSYCHIATRIC or ADDICTIVE DISORDERS.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
“Comparing Two web-based Smoking Cessation Programs: Randomized- Controlled Trial” By: McKay et. Al.
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Orienting Attention to Semantic Categories T Cristescu, JT Devlin, AC Nobre Dept. Experimental Psychology and FMRIB Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford,
A Pain by any other Name (Rejection, Exclusion, Ostracism) still Hurts the Same Mathew D. Leiberman and Naomi I. Eisenberger By: Shauna Halaharvi.
Learning. What is Learning? Acquisition of new knowledge, behavior, skills, values, preferences or understanding.
Drug abuse liability is associated with higher reward-sensitivity: An fMRI study using the Monetary Incentive Delay task C. Corbly, T. Kelly, Y. Jiang,
Neural correlates of risk sensitivity An fMRI study of instrumental choice behavior Yael Niv, Jeffrey A. Edlund, Peter Dayan, and John O’Doherty Cohen.
Neural Activation for Emotional Induction: Differential Responses as a Function of Impulsivity and Reward-Sensitivity C.E.Benca 1, H.R. Collins 1, C.R.
Does the brain compute confidence estimates about decisions?
Introduction Individuals with ASD show inhibitory control deficits that may be associated with the disabling repetitive behaviors characterizing this disorder.
1 Addiction to tobacco Robert West University College London November
Date of download: 7/6/2016 Copyright © 2016 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. From: Disrupted Ventromedial Prefrontal Function, Alcohol.
Reward vs. Punishment: An fMRI Analysis Approach to Identifying the Neural Substrates of Motivation and Cognitive Control Ya’el C. Courtney, Debbie Yee,
Robert West University College London WCTOH Washington 2006
Cognitive Biomarker of MS
Neurofeedback of beta frequencies:
Behavioural and pharmacological approaches to treating smokers
EMA Outcome Measures in a Clinical Trial
Paridon KN, Turner K, Nevison CM, Bristow M, Timmis MA
Professor Greg Murray, PhD, FAPS
Keimyung University, Yang-Tae Kim
Visual Stress and Cognition: An investigation of the impacts of visual stress on lexical decision making Joshua Gietzen, Yu-Chi Tai, PhD, John R. Hayes,
Gaming disorder and ICD-11 beta
Evidence of Inhibitory Processing During Visual Search
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Luke Clark, Andrew J. Lawrence, Frances Astley-Jones, Nicola Gray 
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology
The Brain on Drugs: From Reward to Addiction
Cognitive Mechanisms of Nicotine on Visual Attention
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Social Manipulation of Preference in the Human Brain
Damian M. Herz, Baltazar A. Zavala, Rafal Bogacz, Peter Brown 
Volume 74, Issue 3, Pages (May 2012)
Ronald Keiflin, Patricia H. Janak  Neuron 
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Wallis, JD Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute UC, Berkeley
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Memory aging and brain maintenance
Circuitry of self-control and its role in reducing addiction
A Comparison of Cues for Auditory Motion Judgments
Megan E. Speer, Jamil P. Bhanji, Mauricio R. Delgado  Neuron 
Selection, Integration, and Conflict Monitoring
Myers’ EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY (6th Ed)
BACKGROUND & SIGNIFICANCE SUMMARY & FUTURE DIRECTIONS
David Badre, Bradley B. Doll, Nicole M. Long, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Volume 62, Issue 4, Pages (May 2009)
Matthew T. Sutherland, Elliot A. Stein  Trends in Molecular Medicine 
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
Social Attention and the Brain
The Neural Circuitry of a Broken Promise
Orbitofrontal Cortex as a Cognitive Map of Task Space
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Volume 61, Issue 6, Pages (March 2009)
Presentation transcript:

Brain Connectivity Lab Brain activity following positive and negative performance feedback among abstinent smokers Jessica Flannery Graduate Student Department of Psychology Florida International University Neuroinformatics and Brain Connectivity Lab http://neurolab.fiu.edu Neuroinformatics and Brain Connectivity Lab

Background Why do people continue to smoke? Nicotine withdrawal symptoms, include: - anxiety, irritability ( sensitivity to negative outcomes) reward dysregulation ( sensitivity to positive outcomes) Habenula: epithalmic nucleus linked with anxiety, reward, cognitive & motor functions Inhibits dopaminergic cell firing Possess a high density of nAChRs activity hypothesized to contribute to a hypo-dopaminergic state during nicotine withdrawal Questions of interest: *Task effects: Negative outcomes  Hb activity? Group effects: Increased Hb activity in smokers? Drug effects: Hb activity reduced by drugs? Carroll et al. (2015) Addiction Biol. Fedota et al. (2015) Neuropsychopharm. Ullsperger et al., 2003 J Neuro Savitz et al., 2011 Biol. Psychiat.

Performance Feedback Task Motion Prediction Task Probe Hb, insula, ACC and striatal activity Dynamically adaptive (error rates ~35%) Participants: *24 smokers 20 non-smokers 200 trials FIG. 1: (A) Anticipated drug-effects. (B) Task feedback. Task. To probe Hb, insula, ACC, and VS functioning, participants completed a “motion prediction task” [9] in which they were presented with positive and negative feedback. Participants predicted which of two moving balls, starting from different locations and traveling at different speeds, would first reach a finish line after viewing a clip of the balls’ motion. Task difficulty was dynamically-adapted to maintain error rates at ~35% so that participants remained uncertain about their performance until feedback delivery. Feedback was delivered in a two-factor design. Participant RESPONSEs (correct vs. error) were followed by FEEDBACK (informative vs. non-informative) that either did or did not provide information about trial outcomes (FIG. 1B). Task-related regions are anticipated to show differential activations on error-trials versus correct-trials followed by informative, but not non-informative feedback (i.e., a RESPONSE x FEEDBACK interaction). Participant RESPONSES (correct vs. error) followed by FEEDBACK (informative vs. non-informative)

Behavioral Results Response type: -Correctly: 59.6 ± 1.1% Erroneously: 35.6 ± 0.2% No response: 4.8 ± 1.0% Reaction Time: difficulty manipulation was effective: RT’s slower for Erroneous responses (620 ± 17ms) than for Correct responses (583 ± 17ms) making sure that the difficulty was tailored to each individual such that high uncertainty about whether the response was correct was induced evaluation of the responses and strategy adjustment, participants were dependent on the feedback. Indicating that the dynamically-adaptive task effectively maintained equal numbers of trials across sessions and participants. participants responded correctly on 59.6 ± 1.1%, erroneously on 35.6 ± 0.2%, and gave no response on 4.8 ± 1.0% of the trials (Fig. 2A). Reaction times were slower for erroneous responses (620 ± 17ms) than for correct responses, suggesting that the difficulty manipulation was effective (583 ± 17 ms), F(1,22) = 63.9, p < 0.001 (Fig. 2B).

Imaging Results (task effects) Structural Habenula 2 24 N subjects R L R L y = -24 y = -24 Anterior Cingulate Ventral Striatum x = 6 Initial assessment of task-related effects in smokers confirmed differential activations following positive versus negative feedback in the Hb, insula, ACC, and VS. Specifically, a whole-brain RESPONSE x FEEDBACK interaction analysis (pcorrected < 0.01) indicated that Hb activity was increased on error-trials followed by informative (negative) feedback relative to correct-trials followed by informative (positive) feedback (FIG. 2BC). No difference was observed in Hb activity on error- versus correct-trials followed by non-informative feedback. Critically, this functional activity was localized within the same area identified when overlapping the structurally-defined Hb coordinates from each smoker’s anatomical MRI (FIG. 2AB). Similar effects were observed in the insula and ACC. In contrast, VS activity was increased on correct-trials followed by informative (positive) feedback relative to error-trials followed by informative (negative) feedback (FIG. 3). R L Anterior insula y = 8 z = -4 correct > error error > correct Whole-brain Response X Feedback interaction pcorrected < 0.01 R L

Conclusions & Future Work Task Effects: Habenula (insula, ACC) activity increased following negative feedback Ventral striatum activity increased following positive feedback. Probe of brain regions implicated in nicotine withdrawal Group effects: abstinent smokers (vs. nonsmokers) will show elevated Hb, insula, and ACC activity and decreased VS activity Drug effects: varenicline (pharmacological smoking cessation aid) & nicotine administered to abstinent smokers: decrease Hb, insula, and ACC activity following negative feedback increase VS activity following positive feedback. Hb activity was increased on error-trials followed by informative (negative) feedback relative to correct-trials followed by informative (positive) feedback Administer nicotine (PATCH) and varenicline (PILL) alone and in combination to abstinent smokers AND nonsmokers.

Acknowledgements Nursing Debi Zimmerman Kathy Demuth Sue Baskin Joyce Lutz Becky Price Bill Mantler Barb Glover Anita Hagan Janet Kivet Mary Kay Willing Carol Ward Karen McCullough NBC Lab Matt Sutherland Angie Laird Michael Riedel Jessica Bartley Dane Marguglio Kailey MacNamara Katie Bottenhorn Ranjita Poudel Taylor Salo Jason Hays Karina Falcone Stephanie Rosas NIDA-K01DA037819 (MTS) NIMH-R01MH074457 (ARL) NSF-DRL-1420627 (ARL) NRB (NIDA-IRP) Eliscia Smith Angie Neal Allison Carroll Loretta Spurgeon Kim Slater Betty Jo Salmeron Carolina Smith Mary Lee Hong Gu Lolita Butler Tom Ross Elliot Hong Yihong Yang Elliot Stein Recruiting Margaret Kroen Shannon Pfistner Jason Dietz