Vertex recognition and the deficiencies of linear perspective in art

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Undefined terms in Geometry
Advertisements

This is a painting from the 14th Century
12-1 Representations of Three-Dimensional Figures
Descriptive Geometry:
Geometry Formulas in Three Dimensions
AutoCAD 2D_I Module 5: Orthographic Projection. Module Objectives identify surfaces in two-dimensional views from a given three-dimensional views. Demonstrate.
WAWERU HANNAH NYAKIO B02/53847/2012. PERSPECTIVE DRAWING  Perspective drawing is a drawing technique used to illustrate dimension through a flat surface.
Geometry Thru Composition. rectangles Using rectangles is a close likeness to Rule of Thirds. However, rather than keeping each section of your frame.
ISOMETRICS Isometric means “equal in measure” and refers to the fact that the three receding axes are tilted at 30°. Isometric drawings are constructed.
EMSE 3123 Math and Science in Education
Review of Geometric Shapes
Space Space is basically divided into 3 parts: Foreground, Middle Ground and Background Generally, the background area is considered to be the upper 1/3.
Look at page 193 in your explorations book. Ignore the letters--they are not used for this. Each figure is made up of 5 squares that may or may not be.
Two-Point Perspective
SECTIONAL VIEWS.
How artists use perspective to show space
Unit 16: Engineering Drawing for Technicians Third angle projection
Drafting is the Language of Technology 3.40 BTS Taking your ideas and making them permanent…..
12.1 Exploring Solids.
Solid Geometry Student Expectations 7 th Grade: 7.3.6C Use properties to classify three- dimensional figures, including pyramids, cones, prisms, and.
1.1 Patterns and Inductive Reasoning
Linear Perspective in Visual Arts Mrs. Love. LINEAR PERSPECTIVE.
What is this type of drawing called? Perspective Drawing Linear Perspective: is a system for drawing 3-D space on a 2-D surface by following the rule that.
Properties of Transformations. Translate Figures and Use Vectors Translation: moves every point of a figure the same distance in the same direction Image:
ISOMETRIC DRAWINGS LECTURE NO
Drawing and Rendering Techniques
Section 1.1 Nets and Drawings for Visualizing Geometry
Perspective Drawing One-Point Perspective.
Showing Space in Art During the Renaissance, artists became very interested in making two-dimensional artworks look three-dimensional. "linear perspective"
Nets and Drawings for Visualizing Geometry
Representations of Three-Dimensional Figures
Perspective.
One-Point Perspective
Space The Elements of Art TheVirtualInstructor.com.
Geometric Solids.
Perspective Linear Perspective: creating the illusion of 3-dimensional space on a 2-dimensional surface. A horizon line, vanishing point and perspective.
One-Point Perspective
One-Point Perspective
One-Point Perspective
Architectural Drafting
One-Point Perspective
Two-Point Perspective
REVIEW: Match the vocabulary word up with the definition that best fits. Leave blank any you do not know.
Auxiliary Views Chapter 7.
One-Point Perspective
Circles in Perspective - Elliptical Shapes
Representations of Three-Dimensional Figures
Turn to Page S.76 Describe what you know about the triangle listed to the left. What type of triangle is it? What are the labels A , B , and C ? What are.
Lesson 54 Representing Solids.
One-Point Perspective
Linear Perspective Drawing
DEPICTING THREE-DIMENSIONAL SPACE
Sketching Techniques and Practice!
Warm Up Problem of the Day Lesson Presentation Lesson Quizzes.
Linear Perspective the appearance of things relative to one another
Representations of Three-Dimensional Figures
Two-Point Perspective
One-Point Perspective
One-Point Perspective
One-Point Perspective Drawing and Space
One-Point Perspective
G13 Reflection and symmetry
3-D Views of Solid Figures
What Element of art is MOST prevalent in this image?
SPACE The Illusion of Depth
One-Point Perspective
14. The Duck-Rabbit Illusion
Presentation transcript:

Vertex recognition and the deficiencies of linear perspective in art James Geary • University of Kent • jbg4@kent.ac.uk THE PROBLEM: LEONARDO’S PERSPECTIVE THE SOLUTION: RECOGNITION-BY-COMPONENTS In order to solve the problem of the Virgin’s book we will use the cognitive theory of Recognition-by-Components, proposed by psychologist Irving Biederman. Biederman argues that we recognise objects by decomposing them into a limited number of simple three-dimensional shapes, or ‘Geons’, such as cones, cubes and spheres. Geons can describe a great number of figures; for example, a sphere on a cone can describe an ice-cream. The small number of Geons makes for easy and thus quick recognition. The aspect of Recognition-by-Components theory that is of interest here is Biederman’s theory that the recognition of Geons is facilitated by vertices. Biederman found evidence for this by showing volunteers selections from Figure 4. The volunteers identified the objects when shown the middle column than the right-most column. Biederman classifies vertices into three types: the arrow-vertex (the vertex on an external edge of an object), the Y-vertex (the vertex on an internal edge of an object), and the T-vertex (the vertex that appears in segmentation and occlusion) (Figure 5). By the recognition of vertices, the viewer gains information about the shape of objects, and whether or not an object is in front or behind any other object. Consider for a moment Leonardo da Vinci and Andrea del Verrocchio’s Annunciation (Figure 1). Note that the painting is formed with perfect linear perspective. We take it for granted that, due to the physical properties and geometry of the eye, linear perspective gives the most lifelike way to depict a scene in three-dimensional space. If we now turn to Figure 3, Giotto’s Meeting at the Golden Gate, we see that the linear perspective is badly composed. The horizontal lines at the top of the two towers do not line up, and the rusticated blocks at the bottom of the towers do not recede into space. Despite these failings, however, Giotto’s buildings have a solid three-dimensionality, unlike the Virgin’s book in the Leonardo/Verrocchio. Figure 1 Leonardo da Vinci and Andrea del Verrocchio Annunciation c.1472–1475 In the Annunciation, though, despite the linear perspective being perfectly composed, the Virgin’s book appears curiously two-dimensional (Figure 2). Figure 3 Giotto di Bondone Meeting at the Golden Gate c.1305 We are thus left with a conundrum: how can the correctly constructed linear perspective of the Virgin’s book fail to provide a life-like three-dimensionality, whereas the buildings drawn with Giotto’s poor perspective succeed? Figure 4 Drawing of objects with sections left out, to illustrate Recognition-by-Components theory. From Irving Biederman. ‘Recognition-by-Components: A Theory of Human Image Understanding.’ Psychological Review 94 (1987): 115–147: 135 Figure 5 Biederman’s three vertices. Diagram by the author Figure 2 Leonardo da Vinci and Andrea del Verrocchio Annunciation c.1472–1475 (detail)

CONCLUSION: SOLVING THE PROBLEM OF THE ANNUNCIATION AN EXAMPLE IN ART: HIERONYMUS BOSCH CONCLUSION: SOLVING THE PROBLEM OF THE ANNUNCIATION An example of how Recognition-by-Components theory can be applied to art involves one of the early paintings of Hieronymus Bosch (c.1450–1516), Adoration of the Magi (Figure 6, TOP). The overpainted picture now looks more three-dimensional, but its three-dimensionality is still weak. The only information occlusion provides is that an objects in front or behind another; it does not properly depict volumetric form. The vertices that describe volumetric form are the arrow and the Y, and thus in order to depict volumetric form an artist should use one of these. How has Bosch failed to include one of the other vertices? We might note that the vertex at the bottom of the gable nearest to the picture plane should be an arrow-vertex, thus providing a depiction of volumetric form. However, this is not the case. The bottom edge of the gable and the bottom edge of the thatch instead meet together to form a straight line, thus leaving the corner of the roof without a defining arrow-vertex. We should note that in fact the two lines discussed do not quite make a straight line. However, that we would perceive the two lower edges of the roof as one line, rather than two non-parallel lines, can be demonstrated by an optical phenomenon known as the ‘hypotenuse illusion’, which we see illustrated in Figure 7. The shape in Figure 7 looks like a right-angled triangle, but in fact its hypotenuse is made up of two separate non-parallel lines; the shape is in fact four-sided. The technique of the cut-away depiction of the building is successful in its role of aiding the narrative, but is less successful in its depiction of volumetric form. (Bosing, W. (1994). Bosch: Between Heaven and Hell. New York: Taschen.) The notion of the vertex from Recognition-by-Components theory can explain this lack of success. As we saw in Figure 5, one way that vertices can indicate space is by occlusion. Occlusion occurs when one object partially obscures another object, thus showing that the first object is in front of the second object. Recognition-by-Components theory posits that this is indicated by a T-vertex. We should note that there is only weak occlusion in the Bosch painting, as the ‘overpainting’ of Figure 6 (MIDDLE) demonstrates. Figure 6 (MIDDLE) reinserts the central support of the building, which the artist left out to help form the ‘cutaway’ theme. We can note the changes made in Figure 6 (MIDDLE) make the painting more three-dimensional by increasing the number of T-vertices. In a similar way we perceive the bottom edge of the roof as one straight line. Thus the gable’s bottom vertex is not best labelled an arrow-vertex, for it lacks the necessary clear angle of an arrow-vertex. Figure 8 shows this further: the top picture shows the roof uncorrected, while the bottom picture has been overpainted with a clear arrow-vertex. Figure 6(RIGHT) shows this in the full painting. There are many of these vertices in the overpainted picture, including one made with the central support and St Joseph’s arm, one made with the central support and the arm of the green-robed Magi, and a number made with the central support and the folds of the Virgin’s robe. Figure 6 Hieronymus Bosch Adoration of the Magi c.1500–1550. Additions by the author TOP: no additions MIDDLE: with central post added BOTTOM: with modified gable The reason for the lack of three-dimensionality of the Annunciation’s Virgin’s book can be seen in Figures 9 and Figure 10. The depiction of the book is similar to Bosch’s roof by being constructed predominately using one straight line. It fails to produce a clear vertex, thus explaining why the three-dimensionality of the book is weak. If we now turn to Giotto’s Meeting at the Golden Gate (Figure 11), we notice that the towers have a range of all three types of vertices, hence providing a strong depiction of volumetric form. Figure 9 Leonardo da Vinci and Andrea del Verrocchio Annunciation c.1472–1475 (detail), with a diagram describing the representation of space. Diagram by the author Figure 11 Giotto di Bondone Meeting at the Golden Gate c.1305 (detail), with vertices added. Additions by the author Figure 7 Hypotenuse Illusion. Diagram by the author Figure 10 Leonardo da Vinci and Andrea del Verrocchio Annunciation c.1472–1475 (detail), with vertices added. Additions by the author Figure 8 Hieronymus Bosch Adoration of the Magi c.1500–1550 (detail). Additions by the author