Volume 26, Issue 20, Pages (October 2016)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 2 Exploring Data with Graphs and Numerical Summaries
Advertisements

Cancer drugs in 16 European countries, Australia, and New Zealand: a cross-country price comparison study  Dr Sabine Vogler, PhD, Agnes Vitry, PhD, Zaheer-Ud-Din.
Teddy A. Wilkin, Ben C. Sheldon  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 17, Pages (August 2015)
Volume 21, Issue 8, Pages (April 2011)
Germán Sumbre, Graziano Fiorito, Tamar Flash, Binyamin Hochner 
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Spatial Memory: Mice Quickly Learn a Safe Haven
Ocean Depths: The Mesopelagic and Implications for Global Warming
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
From Compromise to Leadership in Pigeon Homing
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
EEG Responses to Visual Landmarks in Flying Pigeons
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Volume 17, Issue 24, Pages (December 2007)
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages R1152-R1153 (November 2017)
Ingvars Birznieks, Richard M. Vickery  Current Biology 
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Walking Straight into Circles
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 26, Issue 3, Pages (February 2016)
Marianne Elias, Colin Fontaine, F.J. Frank van Veen  Current Biology 
Box Jellyfish Use Terrestrial Visual Cues for Navigation
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
Volume 27, Issue 5, Pages (March 2017)
Volume 25, Issue 21, Pages (November 2015)
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Jianing Yu, David Ferster  Neuron 
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Evolution of a Behavioral Shift Mediated by Superficial Neuromasts Helps Cavefish Find Food in Darkness  Masato Yoshizawa, Špela Gorički, Daphne Soares,
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Marian Stewart Bartlett, Gwen C. Littlewort, Mark G. Frank, Kang Lee 
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Volume 24, Issue 21, Pages (November 2014)
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Elise Facer-Childs, Roland Brandstaetter  Current Biology 
Stephanie C. Weber, Clifford P. Brangwynne  Current Biology 
Volume 26, Issue 5, Pages (March 2016)
Single-Molecule Tracking of Collagenase on Native Type I Collagen Fibrils Reveals Degradation Mechanism  Susanta K. Sarkar, Barry Marmer, Gregory Goldberg,
Steven K. Schwartz, William E. Wagner, Eileen A. Hebets 
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Benjamin Pettit, Zsuzsa Ákos, Tamás Vicsek, Dora Biro  Current Biology 
The Atomistic Mechanism of Conformational Transition in Adenylate Kinase: A TEE-REX Molecular Dynamics Study  Marcus B. Kubitzki, Bert L. de Groot  Structure 
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Volume 13, Issue 9, Pages (September 2005)
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Global Compression Reorients Cortical Microtubules in Arabidopsis Hypocotyl Epidermis and Promotes Growth  Sarah Robinson, Cris Kuhlemeier  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages (July 2015)
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
A Visual Sense of Number
Basal bodies Current Biology
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Volume 26, Issue 20, Pages 2800-2804 (October 2016) Honeybees Learn Landscape Features during Exploratory Orientation Flights  Jacqueline Degen, Andreas Kirbach, Lutz Reiter, Konstantin Lehmann, Philipp Norton, Mona Storms, Miriam Koblofsky, Sarah Winter, Petya B. Georgieva, Hai Nguyen, Hayfe Chamkhi, Hanno Meyer, Pawan K. Singh, Gisela Manz, Uwe Greggers, Randolf Menzel  Current Biology  Volume 26, Issue 20, Pages 2800-2804 (October 2016) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013 Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Representative Flight Trajectories of Bees Displaced after One Short-Range Orientation Flight The flight trajectories of the short-range orientation flights are shown in red. The squares mark the release sites, and the dots on flight paths are the positions given by radar (usually every 3 s). Black dots mark the homing points, the locations at which the bee performed straight homing flights. The position of the hive is marked by the triangle. (A) Flight trajectories of a bee that was displaced into two different directions. The bee did not return to the hive from the second release site (green flight trajectory). The cross marks the location where the bee dropped to ground. (B) Flight trajectories of a bee that was displaced only once. See also Figures S1 and S3. Current Biology 2016 26, 2800-2804DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Homing Performance and Locations of Homing Points of Bees Displaced after One Short-Range Orientation Flight (A) Homing performance determined by the time bees needed to return from a release site to the hive and the straightness of the whole flight, where a value of 1 would indicate a straight flight back to the hive. (B) The locations of homing points (hp) determined by their distance to the hive and their angle to the orientation flight (O). The calculated visual catchment area of the hive is indicated by the dashed line. See also Figures S1 and S2 and Tables S1 and S2. Current Biology 2016 26, 2800-2804DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Representative Flight Trajectories of Bees Displaced after One Long-Range Orientation Flight The flight trajectories of the long-range orientation flights are shown in red, those after displacement to an explored area in blue, and those after displacement to an unexplored area in green. The squares mark the release sites, and the dots on flight paths are the positions given by radar (usually every 3 s). Black dots mark the homing points, the locations at which the bee performed straight homing flights. The position of the hive is marked by the triangle. (A) This bee did not return to the hive after displacement to an unexplored area. The cross marks the location where the bee dropped to ground. (B and C) Flight examples of bees that returned to the hive from the respective release site. See also Figures S1 and S3. Current Biology 2016 26, 2800-2804DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Homing Performance and Locations of Homing Points of Bees Displaced after One Long-Range Orientation Flight (A) Homing performance was determined by the time bees needed to return from a release site to the hive and the straightness of the whole flight, where a value of 1 would indicate a straight flight back to the hive (n = 40). (B) The locations of homing points (hp) were determined by their distance to the hive and their angle (γ) to the orientation flight (O; see inset). Homing points far from the respective orientation flight and rather close to the hive indicate guidance by the hive, and those far from the hive and close to the respective orientation flight are guided by the memory from the orientation flight. The curve is based on the analysis of the displayed homing points and was used to classify explored (below the curve) and unexplored (above the curve) areas. (C) Time bees needed to return from release sites located in areas explored or not explored during the long-range orientation flight to the hive. (D) Straightness of the whole flight, where a value of 1 would indicate a straight flight back to the hive, after displacements to areas explored or not explored during the long-range orientation flight. Boxplots show the median (black line), interquartile range (box), minimum and maximum value within 1.5 times the interquartile range of the box (whiskers), and outliers (circles). The number in each boxplot gives the sample size. Statistics were determined via a linear mixed-effects model (LMM); significant differences (p < 0.05) are marked by asterisks. See also Figures S1 and S2 and Tables S1 and S2. Current Biology 2016 26, 2800-2804DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2016 26, 2800-2804DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.013) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions