Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: /9.5.31 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Chess players' eye movements reveal rapid recognition of complex visual patterns: Evidence from a chess-related visual search task Journal of Vision.
Advertisements

From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
From: Three-Dimensional Analysis of a Concentrated Solar Flux
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Focus information is used to interpret binocular images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccades
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Why the long face? The importance of vertical image structure for biological “barcodes” underlying face recognition Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):25.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Binocular visual acuity for the correction of spherical aberration in polychromatic and monochromatic light Journal of Vision. 2014;14(2):8. doi: /
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Figure Legend: statistical inference
From: Three-dimensional motion aftereffects reveal distinct direction-selective mechanisms for binocular processing of motion through depth Journal of.
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(10):16. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;6(4):5. doi: /tvst Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: Cause and effect
Presentation transcript:

From: An examination of binocular reading fixations based on sentence corpus data Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi:10.1167/9.5.31 Figure Legend: Schematic visualization of a crossed fixation with exaggerated horizontal and vertical disparities. Three-dimensional view from behind the monitor. The blue line represents the principal line of sight for the left eye, the red line for the right eye. The shortest distance between these two lines is marked in black. The middle point on this line defines the binocular fixation point in 3-D space. For comparison, the single black dot marks the binocular fixation point determined in 2-D space, see Footnote 5. Date of download: 11/11/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.