From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Figure Legend: From: Human grasp point selection
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Focus information is used to interpret binocular images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Central Glaucomatous Damage of the Macula Can Be Overlooked by Conventional OCT Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Analyses Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech..
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: ERP P1-N1 changes associated with Vernier perceptual learning and its location specificity and transfer Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):19. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Nonlinearities in color coding: Compensating color appearance for the eye's spectral sensitivity Journal of Vision. 2006;6(9):12. doi: /
Date of download: 11/7/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
From: Maximum likelihood difference scales represent perceptual magnitudes and predict appearance matches Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):1. doi: /
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi: /3.8.2.
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Interactive Feature Modeling for Reverse Engineering
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: Cause and effect
From: Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination Journal of Vision. 2004;4(12):1. doi:10.1167/4.12.1 Figure Legend: Change in slant with azimuth. Left panel: Definitions of local slant and distance. A plane is positioned straight ahead of the Cyclopean eye. The surface normal at the intersection of the plane and the head’s median plane is shown. Slant S is the angle between the normal and the Cyclopean line of sight to that point and d is the distance to that point. The azimuth to another point on the plane is γ. The slant with respect to that point is Sγ and the distance is dγ. Middle panel: Slants and local slants for a cue-conflict stimulus. The disparity-specified slant of the stimulus is Sd = −25 deg and the texture-specified slant is St = −10 deg. The local slants at azimuth γ are Sdγ and Stγ. Right panel: Predicted perceived slants across the cue-conflict stimulus if cue weights are determined locally. The thin red line represents the predicted slant if cue weights are fixed across the surface. The thick red line segments represent the predicted changes in slant if the weights are determined locally. The surface should appear concave in this case. Date of download: 11/11/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.