Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):27. doi: /8.4.27 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Early interference of context congruence on object processing in rapid visual categorization of natural scenes Journal of Vision. 2008;8(13):11.
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rational Interpolation of Car Motions
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Salience of unique hues and implications for color theory
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):10. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals depend on the alignment of the first and second harmonics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):3.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9.
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
From: Temporal eye movement strategies during naturalistic viewing
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: statistical inference
From: Maximum likelihood difference scales represent perceptual magnitudes and predict appearance matches Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):1. doi: /
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Copyright © 2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(7):5. doi: /3.7.5 Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion Journal of Vision.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
Date of download: 12/27/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
From: Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry
A Neural Signature of Divisive Normalization at the Level of Multisensory Integration in Primate Cortex  Tomokazu Ohshiro, Dora E. Angelaki, Gregory C.
Presentation transcript:

From: The role of motion capture in an illusory transformation of optic flow fields Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):27. doi:10.1167/8.4.27 Figure Legend: Target flow speed dependence of the horizontal (○) and vertical (◊) focus shifts in the rotary OFI (a) and the radial OFI (b) in three participants (rows). The horizontal axes represent the speed of the target flow, expressed in spin rate (Hz) for the rotary flow, and simulated translation speed (m/s) in the radial flow conditions. Positive horizontal shift values indicate shifts in the direction of the inducer. Positive vertical shift values indicate shifts at an angle of 90° in the counterclockwise direction with respect to the inducer direction. Each shift is collected with inducer speeds of 5, 0, and −5°/s. In the rotary OFI (a), horizontal inducer motion caused vertical shifts (◊) in all subjects. These shifts were positive in clockwise flow and negative in counterclockwise flow. Significant horizontal shifts (○) were also observed in all participants. Both the vertical and the horizontal shifts were greatest at low target flow speeds. This comes to expression also in bar graph (c), which shows the parameters obtained by fitting Equation 2 to the vertical shifts and Equation 3 to the horizontal shifts. The parameters b y and b x from the spin rate R term are significantly non-zero in all three subjects, whereas the offsets a y and a x are significantly non-zero in only one case. This indicates that both horizontal and vertical shifts in the rotary OFI depend on target flow speed. In the radial OFI (b), the indicated focus positions of expanding and contracting flow were not biased in the vertical direction (◊). Horizontal shifts, however, did occur (○). Here too, shifts were largest at low target flow speeds. Offset c x is significantly non-zero in all participants (d), meaning that the horizontal shift of the FOE was larger than the shift of the FOC. The dashed line in panel b fitted to the horizontal radial focus shifts is the sum of Equations 2 and 3 that were fitted to rotary flow shifts in panel a, see Appendix A for details. Bars per parameter in panels c and d correspond from left to right to participants in panels a and b from top to bottom. All error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The filled black markers correspond to the data in Figure 3. Date of download: 11/13/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.