From: Three-dimensional motion aftereffects reveal distinct direction-selective mechanisms for binocular processing of motion through depth Journal of.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Enhanced Motion Aftereffects in Migraine Are Related to Contrast Sensitivity: Implications for Models of Differences in Precortical/Cortical Function.
Advertisements

From: Chess players' eye movements reveal rapid recognition of complex visual patterns: Evidence from a chess-related visual search task Journal of Vision.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):27. doi: /
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
From: Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccades
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
From: Sandwich masking eliminates both visual awareness of faces and face-specific brain activity through a feedforward mechanism Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):3.
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 10/24/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Figure Legend: From: Transparent layer constancy
Date of download: 10/25/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does print size matter for reading
From: Binocular visual acuity for the correction of spherical aberration in polychromatic and monochromatic light Journal of Vision. 2014;14(2):8. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(9):9. doi: /9.9.9 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
From: How the global layout of the mask influences masking strength
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Cloud Manufacturing: Current Trends and Future Implementations
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Suppression of monocular visual direction under fused binocular stimulation: Evoked potential measurements Journal of Vision. 2005;5(1):4. doi: /5.1.4.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Three-dimensional motion aftereffects reveal distinct direction-selective mechanisms for binocular processing of motion through depth Journal of Vision. 2011;11(10):18. doi:10.1167/11.10.18 Figure Legend: Data and psychometric functions from the (A) 3D adapt, monocular test condition (3D-mono) and (B) the interocular transfer condition (IOT). Under 3D adaptation conditions, one would expect the MAE resulting from monocular adaptation in the tested eye to be partially canceled by the interocular transfer of the (opposing) adaptation in the untested eye. The magnitude of the 3D-mono MAE is smaller than either 2D or 3D MAEs; confirming this expectation. (B) The data resulting from monocular adaptation in one eye and testing in the other eye (i.e., a direct measurement of the interocular transfer). Note that the reversal in the direction of the shift for the “toward” and “away” curves is as expected. Date of download: 11/13/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.