From: What #theDress reveals about the role of illumination priors in color perception and color constancy Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):4. doi:10.1167/17.9.4.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: The Müller-Lyer Illusion: Investigation of a Center of Gravity Effect on the Amplitudes of Saccades Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):11. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Bayesian inference for psychometric functions
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: statistical inference
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(5):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perception of Haidinger Brushes in Macular Disease Depends on Macular Pigment Density and Visual Acuity Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;57(3):
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: What's color got to do with it
Figure Legend: From: Binocular combination of luminance profiles
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: StarTrek Illusion—General object constancy phenomenon?
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Presentation transcript:

From: What #theDress reveals about the role of illumination priors in color perception and color constancy Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):4. doi:10.1167/17.9.4 Figure Legend: (A) Achromatic settings with luminance fixed at 24 \(cd/{m^2}\). (B) The first PC of the dress body matches (\(P{C_b}\)) plotted against the first PC of the achromatic settings at fixed luminance of 24 \(cd/{m^2}\) (\(P{C_f}\)). (C) The first PC of the dress lace matches (\(P{C_l}\)) plotted against the first PC of the achromatic settings at fixed luminance of 24 \(cd/{m^2}\) (\(P{C_f}\)). (D) Achromatic settings with luminance set at luminance of dress body match (and therefore varying across participants). (E) The first PC of the dress body matches (\(P{C_b}\)) plotted against the first PC of the achromatic settings at the luminance of the dress body matches (\(P{C_m}\)). (F) The first PC of the dress lace matches (\(P{C_l}\)) plotted against the first PC of the achromatic settings at the luminance of the dress body matches (\(P{C_m}\)). Red dotted lines are the first PCs of the data. The black dashed line in (A) and (D) is the Planckian locus. Black dashed lines in remaining plots are lines of perfect negative correlation. All markers are pseudocolored according to the corresponding dress body (marker face) and dress lace (marker edge) matches for each participant by converting the CIE \(Yxy\) values of the matches to sRGB for display. Date of download: 11/14/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.