Journal of Vision. 2016;16(2):9. doi: /16.2.9 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Date of download: 6/22/2016 Copyright © 2016 SPIE. All rights reserved. Trim method to select zoom window. Figure Legend: From: Region-of-interest processing.
Advertisements

From: Lower Vitamin D Level and Distinct Tear Cytokine Profile Were Observed in Patients with Mild Dry Eye Signs but Exaggerated Symptoms Trans. Vis. Sci.
From: Early interference of context congruence on object processing in rapid visual categorization of natural scenes Journal of Vision. 2008;8(13):11.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Predicting Future Self-Reported Motor Vehicle Collisions in Subjects with Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma Using the Penalized Support Vector Machine.
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Subjectively homogeneous noise over written text as a tool to investigate the perceptual mechanisms involved in reading Journal of Vision. 2013;13(11):14.
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Date of download: 11/3/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
From: Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response Journal of Vision. 2010;10(2):12. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Estimating predictive stimulus features from psychophysical data: The decision image technique applied to human faces Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):22.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(9):9. doi: /9.9.9 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Saccadic suppression during voluntary versus reactive saccades
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/15/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion Journal of Vision.
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What's color got to do with it
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: StarTrek Illusion—General object constancy phenomenon?
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
From: Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry
Presentation transcript:

From: Pinpointing the peripheral bias in neural scene-processing networks during natural viewing Journal of Vision. 2016;16(2):9. doi:10.1167/16.2.9 Figure Legend: Connectivity properties for category-selective ROIs. (Top) For each ROI, the z-scored weight-versus-eccentricity connectivity profile was averaged across all subjects. All scene-sensitive regions (OPA, PPA, and RSC) show a sharp increase in V1 connectivity as we move outside of 5° of eccentricity. Within PPA, PHC1 and PHC2 show similar profiles, while aPPA has a much less pronounced eccentricity preference. FFA and LOC show the opposite behavior, with connectivity weights concentrated on foveal eccentricities less than 5°. (Middle) We can test whether weights in each ROI increase or decrease with increasing eccentricity by measuring the linear correlation between fitted weights and eccentricity. OPA, PPA, and RSC all connect consistently to more peripheral portions of V1, with the strongest peripheral biases in PPA and especially RSC. There is a detectable peripheral bias throughout all subregions of PPA, though it is weak in aPPA. V1 weights indicating connectivity to FFA and LOC correlate negatively with eccentricity, indicating a foveal connectivity preference. (Bottom) A substantial portion of the variance in visual ROIs can be predicted by a weighted average of V1 time courses, but OPA and PPA are significantly more coupled to V1 than RSC. There is a sharp gradient across PPA, with more posterior subregions highly related to V1 activity and more anterior subregions less predictable from V1. Date of download: 11/14/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.