Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Coordinated Control of Three-Dimensional Components of Smooth Pursuit to Rotating and Translating Textures Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;58(1):
Advertisements

From: Disparity sensitivity in man and owl: Psychophysical evidence for equivalent perception of shape-from-stereo Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):10. doi: /
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(3):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Salience of unique hues and implications for color theory
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):10. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
From: Low-level awareness accompanies “unconscious” high-level processing during continuous flash suppression Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):3. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(6):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi: /3.8.2.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2007;7(11):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Presentation transcript:

From: Voluntary control of long-range motion integration via selective attention to context Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi:10.1167/8.11.18 Figure Legend: Examples of aperture motion and long-range context effects: (a) Single stimulus with diagonal perceived drift (e.g., in direction of red arrow). (b) In the presence of orthogonally oriented contextual gratings (which each on their own may appear to drift diagonally in the direction indicated by the red arrows), all gratings appear to drift together either horizontally (yellow arrow), or (c) vertically (blue arrow), depending on the local drift direction of the context. (d) A combination of both types of context gratings produces spontaneous “multistable” switching between percepts of horizontal, vertical motion (here, in directions orthogonal to the two intersecting axes along which the context pairs are arranged), or local diagonal motion, under free viewing. In the experiments, white dots over the context gratings could be used as attentional cues (as illustrated in d), while more peripheral white dots served as cursors rotating around the center, used by the observer to indicate the vector of currently perceived central motion (in this example diagonal). (e–h) Radial histograms showing the proportional distribution of cursor positions over three bins during free viewing of the stimulus shown above each plot (averaged across 7 subjects with 95% confidence intervals). Note how the presence of horizontal and vertical components in the responses reflect the physical availability of such global motion vectors in the stimulus. Note also the “multistable” outcome for panel d, as shown in panel h. Date of download: 12/3/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.