From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi:10.1167/9.10.20.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Disparity sensitivity in man and owl: Psychophysical evidence for equivalent perception of shape-from-stereo Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):10. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):27. doi: /
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(3):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Transparent layer constancy
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Visual detection of symmetry of 3D shapes
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Date of download: 10/31/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: A short-term memory of multi-stable perception
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/14/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(6):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 12/17/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2007;7(11):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Shape, motion, and optical cues to stiffness of elastic objects
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
From: StarTrek Illusion—General object constancy phenomenon?
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Volume 14, Issue 3, Pages (February 2004)
Presentation transcript:

From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi:10.1167/9.10.20 Figure Legend: a) Schematic representation of a kinetic depth cylinder stimulus. The spatial distribution and speed profile of the dots create the vivid impression of a three-dimensional cylinder rotating around a vertical axis. Without explicit depth cues the rotation direction is ambiguous and bistable. The axis drawn here was not present in the actual stimulus. b) Two coaxially presented stimuli have a strong tendency to be perceived as rotating in the same direction. c) Examples of modal and amodal completion with Kanizsa triangles (Kanizsa, 1979). In the top image, a white triangle appears to float in front of black circles. The illusory triangle surface is constructed through modal completion. The lower image's white triangle is perceived as through a set of apertures in a white ‘foreground’ (amodal completion) while the black shapes are perceived as part of an occluded black ‘background.’ d) Amodal spatial facilitation can resolve local ambiguities. An image of an occluded Schröder's staircase, looked at through three apertures. The image in the middle aperture has ambiguous depth information whereas the left and right are disambiguated by contextual information. If the middle aperture is combined with only one of the two flanking apertures, amodal facilitation disambiguates the depth structure in the middle aperture. Date of download: 12/17/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.