Journal of Vision. 2003;3(7):5. doi: /3.7.5 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;56(8): doi: /iovs Figure Legend:
Advertisements

Copyright © 2010 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
From: The Müller-Lyer Illusion: Investigation of a Center of Gravity Effect on the Amplitudes of Saccades Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):11. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Nonlinear Vibrations and Chaos in Floating Roofs
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 10/21/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Vector subtraction using visual and extraretinal motion signals: A new look at efference copy and corollary discharge theories Journal of Vision.
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(3):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
From: Physiologic statokinetic dissociation is eliminated by equating static and kinetic perimetry testing procedures Journal of Vision. 2016;16(14):5.
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Does print size matter for reading
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
Date of download: 10/27/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals depend on the alignment of the first and second harmonics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):3.
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Date of download: 11/8/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
From: Cross-modal attention influences auditory contrast sensitivity: Decreasing visual load improves auditory thresholds for amplitude- and frequency-modulated.
Journal of Vision. 2005;5(11):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 12/16/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Date of download: 12/18/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion Journal of Vision.
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The absolute threshold of cone vision
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;53(12): doi: /iovs Figure Legend:
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
From: Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry
Effects of Temperature on Heteromeric Kv11.1a/1b and Kv11.3 Channels
Presentation transcript:

From: Dynamics of sensitivity regulation in primate outer retina: The horizontal cell network Journal of Vision. 2003;3(7):5. doi:10.1167/3.7.5 Figure Legend: Peak amplitude of the pulse response and model fits for the cell shown in Figure 7. A. Peak pulse response amplitude plotted against vehicle wave phase (•) for the steady-state condition and for vehicle wave frequencies of 1.22, 4.88, 9.76, and 30.3 Hz. Incremental and decremental pulse responses are shown in the upper and lower plots, respectively. Responses to decremental pulses are larger than those to incremental pulses and more deeply modulated. The degree of modulation on steady backgrounds and 1.22 Hz was similar for both pulse directions, but decreased by ∼10% at 4.88 Hz and ∼30% at 9.76 Hz. Data were fitted with Equation 1 (solid lines) for all conditions except 30.3 Hz. B. Fit parameters as a function of vehicle wave frequency (• increment pulses; O decrement pulses). R0 was frequency independent. B (0.67) was in the range expected from earlier sections. The delay term is equivalent to a lag of 5 ms (incremental pulses) and 8 msec (decremental pulses). Date of download: 12/24/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.