Metrics of time in longitudinal models

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ)
Advertisements

Health and Retirement Study
Copyright EM LYON Par accord du CFC Cession et reproduction interdites Research in Entrepreneurship- The problem of unobserved heterogeneity Frédéric Delmar.
Meta-analysis & psychotherapy outcome research
Analyzing Reliability and Validity in Outcomes Assessment (Part 1) Robert W. Lingard and Deborah K. van Alphen California State University, Northridge.
BPS - 3rd Ed. Chapter 211 Inference for Regression.
Systematic Review Module 7: Rating the Quality of Individual Studies Meera Viswanathan, PhD RTI-UNC EPC.
Section Using Simulation to Estimate Probabilities Objectives: 1.Learn to design and interpret simulations of probabilistic situations.
7.4 DV’s and Groups Often it is desirous to know if two different groups follow the same or different regression functions -One way to test this is to.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
Inferential Statistics Inferential statistics allow us to infer the characteristic(s) of a population from sample data Slightly different terms and symbols.
Data Analysis. Qualitative vs. Quantitative Data collection methods can be roughly divided into two groups. It is essential to understand the difference.
BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 231 Inference for Regression.
Understanding Populations & Samples
Scottish National Burden of Disease, Injuries and Risk Factors study:
Measuring College Value-Added: A Delicate Instrument
Chapter 8 Introducing Inferential Statistics.
BUILDING INTEGRATED HEALTH SERVICE DELIVERY NETWORKS
Attitudes and Intentions
Logic of Hypothesis Testing
Practice & Communication of Science From Distributions to Confidence
How to read a paper D. Singh-Ranger.
Applied Biostatistics: Lecture 2
Biostatistics Case Studies 2016
Experimental Design-Chapter 8
The Diabetic Retinopathy Clinical Research Network
Chapter 5 Sampling Distributions
Chapter 5 Sampling Distributions
evidence based medicine IN THE 21ST CENTURY
Some Epidemiological Studies
From Distributions to Confidence
Alignment Dr. Mary Clisbee
Analyzing Reliability and Validity in Outcomes Assessment Part 1
Deregulating Job Placement in Europe:
11/20/2018 Study Types.
Two Quantitative Variables: Linear Regression
What does science mean to you?
Improving the performance reporting of primary care patient experience
Chapter 5 Sampling Distributions
The Scientific Method and Graphing
Historical Thinking Concepts
INTEGRATED LEARNING CENTER
Financial literacy as an Essential Tool to Empower Older Adult to Manage Health Choices and Later Life Decisions: A 5-City Study Dr Ad Maulod, Centre for.
Does Multilingualism Protect Against Alzheimer’s Disease
Narrative Reviews Limitations: Subjectivity inherent:
Ch. 13. Pooled Cross Sections Across Time: Simple Panel Data.
Topic 1: Statistical Analysis
Basic Practice of Statistics - 3rd Edition Inference for Regression
Young Lives, University of Oxford
Pest Risk Analysis (PRA) Stage 2: Pest Risk Assessment
Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology
What Is a Sampling Distribution?
Inferential Statistics
HCI Evaluation Techniques
Evidence Based Practice
Test Drop Rules: If not:
Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology
LG: I can assess the reliability of a linear model
Interpreting Epidemiologic Results.
Analyzing Reliability and Validity in Outcomes Assessment
Longitudinal Data & Mixed Effects Models
Objectives: To know the different types and varieties of designs that are commonly used in medical researches. To know the characteristics, advantages.
HEC508 Applied Epidemiology
Ch. 13. Pooled Cross Sections Across Time: Simple Panel Data.
2. Sociological Research Methods
Descriptive Statistics
Tiziana Leone and Philipp Hessel LSE health
Elicitation methods Health care demands exceed resource supply
Collecting and Interpreting Quantitative Data
Chapter 5: Sampling Distributions
Presentation transcript:

Metrics of time in longitudinal models Graciela Muniz-Terrera University of Edinburgh Scotland Andrea Piccinin & Scott Hofer University of Victoria Canada The Integrative Analysis of Longitudinal Studies of Aging (IALSA) research network is supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health: 1P01AG043362; 1R01AG026453 and Canadian Institutes of Health Research: 200910MPA Canada-UK Aging Initiative.

Models of change Random effects models are often used to estimate trajectories They require the selection of a metric of time Time may be clocked in different ways: - age (time since birth) - time in study (time past since 1st.occ) But also, for some specific objectives: time to / from event (time to death, dementia diagnosis, time past since stroke, etc.) Complete sample Cases only

Some questions when modelling change: Should we expect consistent results from time in study and age-based models? What factors do we need to think about when choosing a time metric? Should we model time as time in study or as chronological age?

Example Data: Memory and Aging Project (longitudinal study begun 1997 that aims to identify factors associated with maintenance of cognitive health despite AD & other pathologies) Bennett et al. 2012 Oral version of Symbol Digit Modalities test (SDMT): Participants shown a series of symbols, each paired with a number from 1 to 9 and then asked to call out the numbers that match the symbols shown to them one at a time (90s to translate as many symbols as they can)

SDMT scores 10-year span 35+-year span

Three factors to consider when thinking of a time metric: Separation of age differences at study entry (cross sectional (XS) or between person) from aging (longitudinal (LG) or within person) effects Placement of intercept (time origin) & impact of placement on association with potential risk factors Optimal description of trajectory of change

1. Separating XS from LG age effects “Separation of age differences at study entry (cross sectional (XS) or between person) from aging (longitudinal (LG) or within person) effects” Time in study models: common practice to adjust for baseline age to separate XS from longitudinal effects Age based models: not as common to adjust for baseline age (“age is already in the model”) BUT age based models should also be adjusted for baseline age Why?

1. Separating XS from LG age effects Failure to do so: is a form of ecological fallacy (Robinson, 1950): group level relations are mistakenly regarded to be valid at the individual level. implicitly assumes convergence: that cross sectional age differences and longitudinal age changes converge into a common trajectory (Sliwinski, Hoffman, Hofer, 2010). Produces estimates that are a compound of between & within age effects (Sliwinski, Hoffman, Hofer, 2010). Plus: insight into selection bias / healthy participant effect

2. Placement of intercept “Placement of intercept (time origin) & impact of placement on association with potential risk factors” Time in study models have a natural “origin”: the 1st time individuals are exposed to a test. Age-based models require a more arbitrary decision about where to set intercept. - achieved using some form of centering (taking away a certain quantity from observed ages). if age not centered, estimate of “level” would represent expected score at birth (in unconditional models (!))

2. Placement of intercept When arbitrary decisions on centering are made, direct comparison of results across studies is not straightforward. In completely balanced designs, varying the intercept may help gain better understanding of associations between risk factors & outcome. Does not extend directly to studies with missing data. Choice of centering changes associations with risk factors

2. Placement of intercept -choice of centering changes association with risk factors (B. age centered at 79) Time conditional Age conditional centred at 55 Age conditional centred at 75 centred at 85 Fixed effects   Intercept 36.17 (0.28) 66.97 (1.31) 44.96 (0.44) 32.75 (0.32) Baseline age on intercept -0.50 (0.04) 1.41 (0.11) 0.56 (0.04) -0.72 (0.04) Education on intercept 0.99 (0.09) 1.78 (0.25) 1.13 (0.10) 0.93 (0.09) Slope -1.29 (0.05) -1.14 (0.05) -1.14 (0.04) 0.03 (0.003) Baseline age on slope -0.08 (0.006) -0.04 (0.00) -0.04 (0.004) 0.002 (0.00) Education on slope -0.01 (0.01)NS -0.03 (0.01) 0.00 (0.001)NS

Age vs. time in study models: interpretation Age based model centered at 55 Intercept Performance at study entry for a person aged 79 years at study entry Expected performance at age 55 yrs. old for a person aged 79 at study entry Slope (rate of change over entire period) Annual rate of change from study entry for an individual aged 79 at study entry Expected annual rate of change from age 55 for a person aged 79 at study entry Baseline age on intercept Effect of an extra year of age at study entry on performance at study entry Effect of an extra year of age at study entry on expected performance at age 55 (for an individual aged 79 at study entry) Baseline age on slope Effect of an extra year of age at study entry on rate of change Effect of an extra year of age at study entry on rate of change from age 55 for an individual aged 79 at study entry.

3. Shape “Optimal description of trajectory of change” Alignment of scores by Time in study & age may result in different conclusions regarding “shape” of trajectory Time in study: data are not widely spread along x-axis, time represents actual window of observation Chronological age: extrapolation of time beyond period of observation shape may be estimated purely on between persons data

3. Shape Example: Dev Psych, 2002, 38(1): 115-142

In conclusion I Regardless of time metric, inclusion of XS information is crucial for correct inferences Age-based models may be subject to a higher degree of arbitrariness due to wide choice of centering options for intercept As a consequence, associations with risk factors may appear to be inconsistent Shape may reflect XS rather than LG within person information in age-based models

In conclusion II When writing papers, it is essential to report decisions made regarding time metric. If not reported, interpretation of results not easy If age was the choice, then clear statement regarding where intercept was placed is important to place results in context Also, this is important to critically evaluate literature