Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Event-related fMRI Christian Ruff With thanks to: Rik Henson.
Advertisements

Hierarchical Models and
Basis Functions. What’s a basis ? Can be used to describe any point in space. e.g. the common Euclidian basis (x, y, z) forms a basis according to which.
1 st Level Analysis: design matrix, contrasts, GLM Clare Palmer & Misun Kim Methods for Dummies
Experimental Design Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Andrew Holmes Experimental Design Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Andrew Holmes.
FMRI Design & Efficiency Patricia Lockwood & Rumana Chowdhury MFD – Wednesday 12 th 2011.
The General Linear Model Or, What the Hell’s Going on During Estimation?
Event-related fMRI Will Penny (this talk was made by Rik Henson) Event-related fMRI Will Penny (this talk was made by Rik Henson)
Designing a behavioral experiment
Study Design and Efficiency Margarita Sarri Hugo Spiers.
Efficiency in Experimental Design Catherine Jones MfD2004.
Event-related fMRI (er-fMRI) Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 25 March 2009 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research.
Design Efficiency Tom Jenkins Cat Mulvenna MfD March 2006.
Event-related fMRI (er-fMRI) Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 05 November 2008 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research.
The General Linear Model (GLM)
1st level analysis: basis functions and correlated regressors
Parametric modulation, temporal basis functions and correlated regressors Mkael Symmonds Antoinette Nicolle Methods for Dummies 21 st January 2008.
Efficiency – practical Get better fMRI results Dummy-in-chief Joel Winston Design matrix and.
With many thanks for slides & images to: FIL Methods group, Virginia Flanagin and Klaas Enno Stephan Dr. Frederike Petzschner Translational Neuromodeling.
Contrasts (a revision of t and F contrasts by a very dummyish Martha) & Basis Functions (by a much less dummyish Iroise!)
Bayesian Modelling of Functional Imaging Data Will Penny The Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, UCL http//:
Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs.
Wellcome Dept. of Imaging Neuroscience University College London
(Epoch &) Event-related fMRI SPM Course 2002 (Epoch &) Event-related fMRI SPM Course 2002 Christian Buechel Karl Friston Rik Henson Oliver Josephs Wellcome.
(Epoch and) Event-related fMRI Karl Friston Rik Henson Oliver Josephs Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology & Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Event-related fMRI Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London SPM Course Chicago, Oct 2015.
Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs.
Temporal Basis Functions Melanie Boly Methods for Dummies 27 Jan 2010.
Event-related fMRI SPM course May 2015 Helen Barron Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging 12 Queen Square.
Ch. 5 Bayesian Treatment of Neuroimaging Data Will Penny and Karl Friston Ch. 5 Bayesian Treatment of Neuroimaging Data Will Penny and Karl Friston 18.
FMRI Modelling & Statistical Inference Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London SPM Course Chicago, Oct.
Event-related fMRI Christian Ruff With thanks to: Rik Henson.
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping II: GLM for fMRI Alexa Morcom and Stefan Kiebel, Rik Henson, Andrew Holmes & J-B Poline.
The General Linear Model Christophe Phillips SPM Short Course London, May 2013.
Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Group Analyses Guillaume Flandin SPM Course London, October 2016
Original analyses All ROIs
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Statistical Parametric
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping
The General Linear Model
Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs
The General Linear Model (GLM): the marriage between linear systems and stats FFA.
MfD 22/11/17 Kate Ledingham and Arabella Bird
and Stefan Kiebel, Rik Henson, Andrew Holmes & J-B Poline
The SPM MfD course 12th Dec 2007 Elvina Chu
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Sam Ereira Methods for Dummies 13th January 2016
The General Linear Model
fMRI Basic Experimental Design – event-related fMRI.
Experimental Design Christian Ruff With thanks to: Rik Henson
Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping
SPM2: Modelling and Inference
Bayesian Methods in Brain Imaging
The General Linear Model
Hierarchical Models and
Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Event-related fMRI Christian Ruff With thanks to: Rik Henson.
MfD 04/12/18 Alice Accorroni – Elena Amoruso
Bayesian Inference in SPM2
The General Linear Model
Experimental Design Christian Ruff With slides from: Rik Henson
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Probabilistic Modelling of Brain Imaging Data
WellcomeTrust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London
The General Linear Model
The General Linear Model
Presentation transcript:

Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Advantages of Event-related fMRI 1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997) 2. Post hoc / subjective classification of trials e.g, according to subsequent memory (Wagner et al 1998) 3. Some events can only be indicated by subject (in time) e.g, spontaneous perceptual changes (Kleinschmidt et al 1998) 4. Some trials cannot be blocked e.g, “oddball” designs (Clark et al., 2000) 5. More accurate models even for blocked designs? e.g, “state-item” interactions (Chawla et al, 1999)

(Disadvantages of Randomised Designs) 1. Less efficient for detecting effects than are blocked designs (see later…) 2. Some psychological processes may be better blocked (eg task-switching, attentional instructions)

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

BOLD Impulse Response Function of blood oxygenation, flow, volume (Buxton et al, 1998) Peak (max. oxygenation) 4-6s poststimulus; baseline after 20-30s Initial undershoot can be observed (Malonek & Grinvald, 1996) Similar across V1, A1, S1… … but differences across: other regions (Schacter et al 1997) individuals (Aguirre et al, 1998) Brief Stimulus Undershoot Initial Peak

BOLD Impulse Response Early event-related fMRI studies used a long Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) to allow BOLD response to return to baseline However, if the BOLD response is explicitly modelled, overlap between successive responses at short SOAs can be accommodated… … particularly if responses are assumed to superpose linearly Short SOAs are more sensitive… Brief Stimulus Undershoot Initial Peak

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

General Linear (Convolution) Model GLM for a single voxel: Y(t) = x(t)  h(t) +  x(t) = stimulus train (delta functions) x(t) =   (t - nT) h(t) = hemodynamic (BOLD) response h(t) =  ßi fi(t) fi(t) = temporal basis functions Y(t) =   ßi fi (t - nT) +  T 2T 3T ... x(t) h(t)= ßi fi (t) convolution sampled each scan Design Matrix

General Linear Model (in SPM) Auditory words every 20s SPM{F} 0 time {secs} 30 Gamma functions ƒi() of peristimulus time  (Orthogonalised) Sampled every TR = 1.7s Design matrix, X [x(t)ƒ1() | x(t)ƒ2() |...] …

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Temporal Basis Functions Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test

Temporal Basis Functions Finite Impulse Response Mini “timebins” (selective averaging) Any shape (up to bin-width) Inference via F-test

Temporal Basis Functions Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test Gamma Functions Bounded, asymmetrical (like BOLD) Set of different lags

Temporal Basis Functions Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test Gamma Functions Bounded, asymmetrical (like BOLD) Set of different lags “Informed” Basis Set Best guess of canonical BOLD response Variability captured by Taylor expansion “Magnitude” inferences via t-test…?

Temporal Basis Functions

Temporal Basis Functions “Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) Canonical

Temporal Basis Functions “Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) Canonical Temporal

Temporal Basis Functions “Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

Temporal Basis Functions “Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) “Magnitude” inferences via t-test on canonical parameters (providing canonical is a good fit…more later) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

Temporal Basis Functions “Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) “Magnitude” inferences via t-test on canonical parameters (providing canonical is a good fit…more later) “Latency” inferences via tests on ratio of derivative : canonical parameters (more later…) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

(Other Approaches) Long Stimulus Onset Asychrony (SOA) Can ignore overlap between responses (Cohen et al 1997) … but long SOAs are less sensitive Fully counterbalanced designs Assume response overlap cancels (Saykin et al 1999) Include fixation trials to “selectively average” response even at short SOA (Dale & Buckner, 1997) … but unbalanced when events defined by subject Define HRF from pilot scan on each subject May capture intersubject variability (Zarahn et al, 1997) … but not interregional variability Numerical fitting of highly parametrised response functions Separate estimate of magnitude, latency, duration (Kruggel et al 1999) … but computationally expensive for every voxel

Temporal Basis Sets: Which One? In this example (rapid motor response to faces, Henson et al, 2001)… Canonical + Temporal + Dispersion + FIR …canonical + temporal + dispersion derivatives appear sufficient …may not be for more complex trials (eg stimulus-delay-response) …but then such trials better modelled with separate neural components (ie activity no longer delta function) + constrained HRF (Zarahn, 1999)

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Timing Issues : Practical TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s

Timing Issues : Practical TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Stimulus (synchronous) SOA=8s Sampling rate=4s

Timing Issues : Practical TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR Stimulus (asynchronous) SOA=6s Sampling rate=2s

Timing Issues : Practical TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR 2. Random Jitter eg SOA=(2±0.5)TR Stimulus (random jitter) Sampling rate=2s

Timing Issues : Practical TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR 2. Random Jitter eg SOA=(2±0.5)TR Better response characterisation (Miezin et al, 2000) Stimulus (random jitter) Sampling rate=2s

Timing Issues : Practical …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices

Timing Issues : Practical Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t}

Timing Issues : Practical Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices Solutions: 1. Temporal interpolation of data … but less good for longer TRs TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t} Interpolated SPM{t}

Timing Issues : Practical Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices Solutions: 1. Temporal interpolation of data … but less good for longer TRs 2. More general basis set (e.g., with temporal derivatives) … but inferences via F-test TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t} Interpolated SPM{t} Derivative SPM{F}

Timing Issues : Latency Assume the real response, r(t), is a scaled (by ) version of the canonical, f(t), but delayed by a small amount dt: r(t) =  f(t+dt) ~  f(t) +  f ´(t) dt 1st-order Taylor If the fitted response, R(t), is modelled by the canonical + temporal derivative: R(t) = ß1 f(t) + ß2 f ´(t) GLM fit Then canonical and derivative parameter estimates, ß1 and ß2, are such that :  = ß1 dt = ß2 / ß1 (Henson et al, 2002) (Liao et al, 2002) ie, Latency can be approximated by the ratio of derivative-to-canonical parameter estimates (within limits of first-order approximation, +/-1s)

Timing Issues : Latency Delayed Responses (green/ yellow) Canonical Canonical Derivative Basis Functions ß1 ß2 Parameter Estimates ß2 /ß1 Actual latency, dt, vs. ß2 / ß1 Face repetition reduces latency as well as magnitude of fusiform response

Timing Issues : Latency Neural BOLD A. Decreased B. Advanced C. Shortened (same integrated) D. Shortened (same maximum) A. Smaller Peak B. Earlier Onset C. Earlier Peak D. Smaller Peak and earlier Peak

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Design Efficiency T = cTb / var(cTb) Var(cTb) = sqrt(2cT(XTX)-1c) (i.i.d) For max. T, want min. contrast variability (Friston et al, 1999) If assume that noise variance (2) is unaffected by changes in X… …then want maximal efficiency, e: e(c,X) = { cT (XTX)-1 c }-1 = maximal bandpassed signal energy (Josephs & Henson, 1999)

Design Efficiency

Design Efficiency HRF can be viewed as a filter (Josephs & Henson, 1999) Want to maximise the signal passed by this filter Dominant frequency of canonical HRF is ~0.04 Hz So most efficient design is a sinusoidal modulation of neural activity with period ~24s (eg, boxcar with 12s on/ 12s off)

Efficiency - Single Event-type Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA

Efficiency - Single Event-type Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin

Efficiency - Single Event-type Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nT

Efficiency - Single Event-type Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nSOAmin Stationary stochastic p(t)=constant<1

Efficiency - Single Event-type Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nT Stationary stochastic p(t)=constant Dynamic stochastic p(t) varies (eg blocked) Blocked designs most efficient! (with small SOAmin)

Efficiency - Multiple Event-types Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA pi(h) Probability of event-type i given history h of last m events With n event-types pi(h) is a nm  n Transition Matrix Example: Randomised AB A B A 0.5 0.5 B 0.5 0.5 => ABBBABAABABAAA... 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Differential Effect (A-B) Common Effect (A+B)

Efficiency - Multiple Event-types Example: Alternating AB A B A 0 1 B 1 0 => ABABABABABAB... 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Permuted (A-B) Alternating (A-B) Example: Permuted AB A B AA 0 1 AB 0.5 0.5 BA 0.5 0.5 BB 1 0 => ABBAABABABBA...

Efficiency - Multiple Event-types Example: Null events A B A 0.33 0.33 B 0.33 0.33 => AB-BAA--B---ABB... Efficient for differential and main effects at short SOA Equivalent to stochastic SOA (Null Event like third unmodelled event-type) Selective averaging of data (Dale & Buckner 1997) 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Null Events (A-B) Null Events (A+B)

SOA=2s SOA=16s A+B Ran A-B Alt A-B

Efficiency - Conclusions Optimal design for one contrast may not be optimal for another Blocked designs generally most efficient with short SOAs (but earlier restrictions and problems of interpretation…) With randomised designs, optimal SOA for differential effect (A-B) is minimal SOA (assuming no saturation), whereas optimal SOA for main effect (A+B) is 16-20s Inclusion of null events improves efficiency for main effect at short SOAs (at cost of efficiency for differential effects) If order constrained, intermediate SOAs (5-20s) can be optimal; If SOA constrained, pseudorandomised designs can be optimal (but may introduce context-sensitivity)

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Nonlinear Model [u(t)] Volterra series - a general nonlinear input-output model y(t) = 1[u(t)] + 2[u(t)] + .... + n[u(t)] + .... n[u(t)] = ....  hn(t1,..., tn)u(t - t1) .... u(t - tn)dt1 .... dtn [u(t)] response y(t) input u(t) Stimulus function kernels (h) estimate

Nonlinear Model Friston et al (1997) kernel coefficients - h SPM{F} SPM{F} testing H0: kernel coefficients, h = 0

Nonlinear Model Friston et al (1997) kernel coefficients - h SPM{F} SPM{F} testing H0: kernel coefficients, h = 0 Significant nonlinearities at SOAs 0-10s: (e.g., underadditivity from 0-5s)

Underadditivity at short SOAs Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Volterra Prediction

Underadditivity at short SOAs Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Volterra Prediction

Underadditivity at short SOAs Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Implications for Efficiency Volterra Prediction

Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response 3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

Example 1: Intermixed Trials (Henson et al 2000) Short SOA, fully randomised, with 1/3 null events Faces presented for 0.5s against chequerboard baseline, SOA=(2 ± 0.5)s, TR=1.4s Factorial event-types: 1. Famous/Nonfamous (F/N) 2. 1st/2nd Presentation (1/2)

Lag=3 . . . Famous Nonfamous (Target)

Example 1: Intermixed Trials (Henson et al 2000) Short SOA, fully randomised, with 1/3 null events Faces presented for 0.5s against chequerboard baseline, SOA=(2 ± 0.5)s, TR=1.4s Factorial event-types: 1. Famous/Nonfamous (F/N) 2. 1st/2nd Presentation (1/2) Interaction (F1-F2)-(N1-N2) masked by main effect (F+N) Right fusiform interaction of repetition priming and familiarity

Example 2: Post hoc classification (Henson et al 1999) SPM{t} R-K SPM{t} K-R Subjects indicate whether studied (Old) words: i) evoke recollection of prior occurrence (R) ii) feeling of familiarity without recollection (K) iii) no memory (N) Random Effects analysis on canonical parameter estimate for event-types Fixed SOA of 8s => sensitive to differential but not main effect (de/activations arbitrary)

Example 3: Subject-defined events (Portas et al 1999) Subjects respond when “pop-out” of 3D percept from 2D stereogram

Example 3: Subject-defined events (Portas et al 1999) Subjects respond when “pop-out” of 3D percept from 2D stereogram Popout response also produces tone Control event is response to tone during 3D percept Temporo-occipital differential activation Pop-out Control

Example 4: Oddball Paradigm (Strange et al, 2000) 16 same-category words every 3 secs, plus … … 1 perceptual, 1 semantic, and 1 emotional oddball

Perceptual Oddball Semantic Oddball Emotional Oddball ~3s WHEAT BARLEY CORN Perceptual Oddball BARLEY OATS PLUG Semantic Oddball RYE RAPE Emotional Oddball HOPS ~3s …

Example 4: Oddball Paradigm (Strange et al, 2000) Right Prefrontal Cortex 16 same-category words every 3 secs, plus … … 1 perceptual, 1 semantic, and 1 emotional oddball 3 nonoddballs randomly matched as controls Conjunction of oddball vs. control contrast images: generic deviance detector Parameter Estimates Controls Oddballs

Example 5: Epoch/Event Interactions (Chawla et al 1999) Interaction between attention and stimulus motion change in V5 Epochs of attention to: 1) motion, or 2) colour Events are target stimuli differing in motion or colour Randomised, long SOAs to decorrelate epoch and event-related covariates Interaction between epoch (attention) and event (stimulus) in V4 and V5 attention to motion attention to colour

THE END

Blocked Randomised Data Model O = Old Words N = New Words O1 O2 O3 N1

Confounds of blocking (Herron et al, 2004) PET studies of “memory retrieval” compared blocks (scans) with different ratios of old:new items… However, the ratio actually affects the difference between old and new items OLD – NEW (Collapse Ratio) Left Inferior Parietal (BA 7/40) Medial Parietal (BA 18/31) (LOW–HIGH) x (OLD-NEW) Right Anterior Prefrontal (BA 10) Left Dorsal Prefrontal (BA 9/46)

Event-Related ~4s R F R = Words Later Remembered F = Words Later Forgotten Event-Related ~4s Data Model

“Oddball” … Time

Blocked Design “Epoch” model “Event” model Data “Epoch” model Model O1 O2 O3 N1 N2 N3 N1 N2 N3 “Event” model O1 O2 O3

A word about down-sampling T=16, TR=2s Scan 1 o T0=16 o T0=9 x2 x3

Epoch vs Events Sustained epoch Series of events => New in SPM2 Epoch vs Events Epochs are periods of sustained stimulation (e.g, box-car functions) Events are impulses (delta-functions) In SPM99, epochs and events are distinct (eg, in choice of basis functions) In SPM2, all conditions are specified in terms of their 1) onsets and 2) durations… … events simply have zero duration Near-identical regressors can be created by 1) sustained epochs, 2) rapid series of events (SOAs<~3s) i.e, designs can be blocked or intermixed … models can be epoch or event-related Boxcar function Sustained epoch Series of events Delta functions Convolved with HRF =>

Epoch vs Events Rate = 1/4s Rate = 1/2s Though blocks of trials can be modelled as boxcars or runs of events… … interpretation of the parameter estimates differs… Consider an experiment presenting words at different rates in different blocks: An “epoch” model will estimate parameter that increases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per block An “event” model may estimate parameter that decreases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per word b=3 b=5 b=11 b=9

BOLD Response Latency (Iterative) Height Peak Delay Onset Delay Numerical fitting of explicitly parameterised canonical HRF (Henson et al, 2001) Distinguishes between Onset and Peak latency… …unlike temporal derivative… …and which may be important for interpreting neural changes (see previous slide) Distribution of parameters tested nonparametrically (Wilcoxon’s T over subjects)

BOLD Response Latency (Iterative) 240ms Peak Delay wT(11)=14, p<.05 0.34% Height Change wT(11)=5, p<.001 No difference in Onset Delay, wT(11)=35 D. Shortened (same maximum) Neural D. Smaller Peak and earlier Peak BOLD Most parsimonious account is that repetition reduces duration of neural activity…

BOLD Response Latency (Iterative) Height Peak Delay Onset Delay Dispersion Different fits across subjects Four-parameter HRF, nonparametric Random Effects (SNPM99) Advantages of iterative vs linear: Height “independent” of shape Canonical “height” confounded by latency (e.g, different shapes across subjects); no slice-timing error 2. Distinction of onset/peak latency Allowing better neural inferences? Disadvantages of iterative: 1. Unreasonable fits (onset/peak tension) Priors on parameter distributions? (Bayesian estimation) 2. Local minima, failure of convergence? 3. CPU time (~3 days for above) FIR used to deconvolve data, before nonlinear fitting over PST Height p<.05 (cor) 1-2 SNPM

Temporal Basis Sets: Inferences How can inferences be made in hierarchical models (eg, “Random Effects” analyses over, for example, subjects)? 1. Univariate T-tests on canonical parameter alone? may miss significant experimental variability canonical parameter estimate not appropriate index of “magnitude” if real responses are non-canonical (see later) 2. Univariate F-tests on parameters from multiple basis functions? need appropriate corrections for nonsphericity (Glaser et al, 2001) 3. Multivariate tests (eg Wilks Lambda, Henson et al, 2000) not powerful unless ~10 times as many subjects as parameters

Efficiency – Detection vs Estimation “Detection power” vs “Estimation efficiency” (Liu et al, 2001) Detect response, or characterise shape of response? Maximal detection power in blocked designs; Maximal estimation efficiency in randomised designs => simply corresponds to choice of basis functions: detection = canonical HRF estimation = FIR

 =  =  = r() s(t) u(t) u(t) h() x(t) u(t) h() x(t) PST (s) Time (s) Time (s) u(t) h() x(t)  = Time (s) PST (s) Time (s) u(t) h() x(t)  = Time (s) PST (s) Time (s)

A B C Peak Dispersion Undershoot Initial Dip