Color Me Bad Amber K. Lupo, M.A., Julie Alvarez, B.S., & Michael A. Zárate, Ph.D. University of Texas at El Paso Hypotheses We hypothesized that participants.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Writing an Introduction
Advertisements

(How do scientists conduct scientific experimentation?)
Learning by example: Exposure to others’ success improves people’s expectations about interracial contact Participants Participants were 60 (39 Females,
Police Perceptions and Rule Rejections By: Chad Roberge Advisors: Ellen S. Cohn, Ph.D. & Alexander Blandina, M.A. Hypothesis Results Discussion Method.
Bringing Home the Bacon: The Role of Implicit Emotional Attitude on Hiring Ex-Offenders Rebecca Lange, Corey K. Fallon, MS, & Peter Chiu, PhD Department.
University of Texas at El Paso
Using Race to Make Sense of Athletic Performance
AICE Psychology Introduction.
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Smarty Pants: The Trivia Game For Everyone!
Nova Southeastern University, College of Psychology and Neuroscience
Better to Give or to Receive?: The Role of Dispositional Gratitude
Occupations & Subfields of Psychology
Module 2 Research Strategies
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Sexiness on Social Media Hurts Men Too
Lesson 3 Ethics and Validity.
Lesson by Ryan Benson, M.A.
Chapter 9: Conducting Experiments
Chapter 6: Social Influence and Group Behavior
Sport-Specific Stressor
Myers’ PSYCHOLOGY (5th Ed)
Parental Affective Reactions to Prolonged Infant Crying: Does Risk Status or Parent Gender Matter? Kreila Cote, Christie Miksys, Sapir Sasson, Jennifer.
Career Portfolios Building Your Own Personal Career Portfolio
MODULE 2 Myers’ Exploring Psychology 5th Ed.
Christian Christopherson and Michael J. Crowley
Scientific Methodology
BIOLOGY NOTES SCIENTIFIC METHODS PART 2 PAGES 13-18
Myers’ EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY (4th Ed)
with Child Sexual Abuse Histories
Influence of Meeting Humor Styles on Meeting Satisfaction
Chapter 2: Where to Start
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Increased/Maintained UO
Charles Chu1, Sohad Murrar2, Evava Pietri1, Rebecca Rosen3
Developing Group Stereotypes Over Time
Unit 1. First Impressions
RECAP Whiteboard relay… Outline and evaluate Milgram’s original obedience study (12)
Getting What They Deserve
ABSTRACT Data randomly collected from Division I collegiate softball athletes (n = 148) and head softball coaches (n = 20) during the fall of 2012 were.
Thinking Like a Scientist
Secret Shopper Training: Odum Library
Module 02 Research Strategies.
Methods of Studying Human Behavior
Henrik Singmann1, Andreas Kappes1 & Gabriele Oettingen1,2
BIOLOGY NOTES SCIENTIFIC METHODS PART 2 PAGES 13-18
Introduction to Science: The Scientific Method
Method Separate subheadings for participants, materials, and procedure (3 marks in total) Participants (1 mark) Include all info provided in the assignment.
Starter Imagine - you did not do as well as you wanted to in a biology test, but your teacher praises you for working hard and trying your best. You feel.
They Did it on Purpose… Or Did They?
Introduction to Psychology Chapter 1
Generating and Refining Research Ideas
Charles N. Elliott, Paul A. Story
Substance versus Presentation of Mathematics Ideas
Scientific Method Steps
by Carl O. Word, Mark P. Zanna, and Joel Cooper
Independent Variables in Experimental Design
Scientific Method (AKA Scientific Problem Solving)
Ch. 1 – Thinking About Communication: Definitions, Models, and Ethics
Conducting experiments
Thinking critically with psychological science
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Self-Understanding: How We Come to Understand Ourselves
BIOLOGY NOTES SCIENTIFIC METHODS PART 2 PAGES 13-18
Scientific Method Basic procedures
Myers’ EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY (6th Ed)
What Are Ethics? What are the objectives?
AS Psychology Research Methods
Presentation transcript:

Color Me Bad Amber K. Lupo, M.A., Julie Alvarez, B.S., & Michael A. Zárate, Ph.D. University of Texas at El Paso Hypotheses We hypothesized that participants will evaluate an authority figure more negatively (i.e., as aggressive) when the authority gives negative feedback on a task and wears black, versus white, clothing. Method   Procedure Participants were recruited to complete a study ostensibly assessing general aptitude. The study consisted of a one-on-one experimental session with a confederate. A trained confederate asked them “common knowledge” mathematical questions and responded with bogus feedback. Fifteen “common knowledge” questions were selected from the State of Texas Assessments of Academic Readiness standardized examination (STAAR; Texas Education Agency, 2015). Participants were asked a total of five questions related to mathematics. Participants were then asked to rate how much they liked the person running the study, to what extent they had guessed on the mathematical questions, and to evaluate the confederate along a series of traits, including rude, mean, aggressive, and weak. Feedback Positive feedback: Good job. You really know your stuff. Wrong. It’s so difficult, I can’t believe you got it right. Correct. You’re one of the only people to get that right. Negative feedback: Bad try. Too bad you don’t know your stuff. It’s so easy, I don’t see why you got it wrong. Everyone else knew that answer but you. Results Liking of Confederate Results revealed a main effect of feedback, F(1, 117) = 13.37, p <.001, such that confederates were rated as more likable in the positive (M = 5.89; SD = 1.29) compared to negative (M = 4.95; SD = 1.43) feedback condition. Guessing Results revealed a main effect of feedback, F(1, 118) = 16.00, p < .0001, such that participants reported guessing their answers more when given negative (M = 4.83; SD = 1.66) versus positive (M = 3.63; SD = 1.79) feedback. Results Cont. Confederate Evaluation The predicted color by feedback interaction was significant, F(1, 118) = 11.27, p < .01, partial η2 = 0.09. In the negative feedback condition, participants perceived the confederate as more negative when the confederate wore black (M = 3.56; SD = 1.04) than when the confederate wore white (M = 2.94; SD = 0.85; t(58) = 2.80, p < .01; d = 0.73). In the positive feedback condition, participants’ rating of the confederate was not significantly different between the black (M = 2.15; SD = 0.61) and the white (M = 2.57; SD = 0.74) conditions. Effect by confederate Both confederates demonstrated this predicted interaction, F(1, 27) = 7.90, p = .0091 and F(1, 76) = 5.12, p = .03, for Confederate 1 and Confederate 2, respectively. Discussion Results supported our hypothesis. Confederates wearing black and giving negative feedback were rated as more aggressive as compared to the white color condition with the same feedback. These findings are contrary to those of Nickels (2008); the current study indicates an authority figure in black is rated less favorably than when wearing white. Results suggest that wearing lighter colors may help law enforcement officers to safely and effectively perform their duties while simultaneously protecting civilians from undue harm It is possible that our confederates unknowingly behaved differently when wearing black versus white. Thus, future studies will control for the authority’s behavior, while only manipulating uniform color. Future studies will also benefit from investigating how behavior is implicitly affected by uniform color. References Frank, M. G., & Gilovich, T. (1988). The dark side of self- and social perception: Black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 74-85. Meier, B. P., Robinson, M.D., Clore, G.L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82-87. Sherman, G.D., & Clore, G.L., (2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, 1019-1025. Texas Education Agency (2015). STAAR released test questions. Retrieved from http://tea.texas.gov/Student_Testing_and_Accountability/Testing/State_of_Texas_Assessments_of_Academic_Readiness_%28STAAR%29/STAAR_Released_Test_Questions/ Abstract This study tested the effects of how an authority figure’s clothing color can affect others’ perception of them. A confederate, wearing either a black or white t-shirt, asked participants (N=122) purported “common knowledge” questions and then provided bogus feedback (negative or positive). Confederates were blind to experimental hypotheses. It was hypothesized that confederates would be perceived more negatively when wearing black clothing and giving negative feedback. Results demonstrated the predicted two-way interaction between clothing color and feedback, such that a confederate wearing black and providing negative feedback was perceived the most negatively. These results suggest that authority figures, such as law enforcement officers, may inadvertently foster more negative interactions with civilians when wearing black uniforms. Introduction Colors can evoke and communicate emotion; darker colors are associated with negativity while lighter colors are associated with positivity. This association is engrained into our culture and occurs implicitly (Meier, Robinson, & Clore, 2004; Sherman & Clore, 2009). Research demonstrates that clothing color can affect social perception, such that persons wearing darker colors are perceived as more aggressive and also behave more aggressively (Frank& Gilovich, 1988). Recently, more cases are appearing of questionable police conduct, and if one performs a Google search for images of “police brutality” many of these images depict officers wearing dark colored uniforms and fewer wearing light colored uniforms. Considering the above research, it makes sense to hypothesize that darker colored uniforms may lead to more negative impressions of law enforcement officers. If this is the case, then officers may be inadvertently subject to more hostile interactions with civilians. The present study aims to test this broad hypothesis.