Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: /17.8.6 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Figure Legend: From: Human grasp point selection
Advertisements

Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;56(8): doi: /iovs Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(13). doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Visual detection of symmetry of 3D shapes
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals depend on the alignment of the first and second harmonics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):3.
From: Systematic Review: Strategies for Using Exercise Therapy To Improve Outcomes in Chronic Low Back Pain Ann Intern Med. 2005;142(9): doi: /
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: Illumination discrimination in real and simulated scenes
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Estimating predictive stimulus features from psychophysical data: The decision image technique applied to human faces Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):22.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
Figure Legend: From: The color lexicon of the Somali language
From: Saccadic suppression during voluntary versus reactive saccades
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
Figure Legend: From: A short-term memory of multi-stable perception
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(1):1. doi: /8.1.1 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(13). doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry
Presentation transcript:

From: On the origin of sensory errors: Contrast discrimination under temporal constraint Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi:10.1167/17.8.6 Figure Legend: A Monte Carlo simulation of the adaptive algorithm. (A) Here, we see Monte-Carlo simulations of the adaptive algorithm over 45 trials, which are marked on the x axis. On the top graphs, we have 10 simulations overlaid on top of one another (separate lines), with the value for β calculated as being most probable shown on the y axis. The dashed black line is the true value of β (βtrue), i.e., the value for the simulated responder. Note that the three graphs at the top have separate values for βtrue, denoted in their titles. For all three graphs, βprior started 40% below βtrue. The three graphs on the bottom show a mean of calculated βprior (solid line) for the 10 simulations above, and the standard error (dashed lines) for the same. These graphs give a better insight on the average course of the convergence. (B) This set of graphs show the same thing as (A), with the only difference being that the starting βprior is set at 40% above the true value of β. Date of download: 12/31/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.