Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Date of download: 9/17/2016 Copyright © 2016 SPIE. All rights reserved. Examples of synthetic simple scenes (in anaglyph 3-D). Each visual stimulus in.
Advertisements

From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
From: Why the long face? The importance of vertical image structure for biological “barcodes” underlying face recognition Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):25.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals depend on the alignment of the first and second harmonics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):3.
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
From: ERP P1-N1 changes associated with Vernier perceptual learning and its location specificity and transfer Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):19. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(16):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Estimating predictive stimulus features from psychophysical data: The decision image technique applied to human faces Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):22.
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What's color got to do with it
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Presentation transcript:

From: The zone of comfort: Predicting visual discomfort with stereo displays Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi:10.1167/11.8.11 Figure Legend: Natural viewing, phoria, and stereo displays. The left panel plots phoria as a function of vergence and focal distance. The abscissa is the vergence response in diopters. The ordinate is the focal stimulus in diopters. The dashed diagonal line represents the vergence and focal distances associated with natural viewing. The purple and blue lines are examples of esophoria and exophoria, respectively. The green line represents the phoria of a typical individual: esophoric at far and exophoric at near. The right panel shows the focal distances for typical stereo devices: mobile devices, desktop displays, television, and cinema. Assuming that the vergence response is accurate, we can plot the focal distances associated with those devices on the same graph. The blue dashed lines indicate stereo content that would be farther than the screen and the purple dashed lines indicate content that would be nearer than the screen. Date of download: 12/31/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.