Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):7. doi: /15.9.7 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Advertisements

From: A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):27. doi: /
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
From: Visual illusions based on single-field contrast asynchronies
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Subjectively homogeneous noise over written text as a tool to investigate the perceptual mechanisms involved in reading Journal of Vision. 2013;13(11):14.
From: Binocular visual acuity for the correction of spherical aberration in polychromatic and monochromatic light Journal of Vision. 2014;14(2):8. doi: /
From: ERP P1-N1 changes associated with Vernier perceptual learning and its location specificity and transfer Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):19. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(16):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response Journal of Vision. 2010;10(2):12. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The color lexicon of the Somali language
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: A short-term memory of multi-stable perception
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
From: Maximum likelihood difference scales represent perceptual magnitudes and predict appearance matches Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: Threshold and Variability Properties of Matrix Frequency-Doubling Technology and Standard Automated Perimetry in Glaucoma Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Design of a Wireless Biological Signal Conditioning System1
From: Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: Cause and effect
From: Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Beyond the classical receptive field: The effect of contextual stimuli Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):7. doi:10.1167/15.9.7 Figure Legend: Hypothetical wiring diagram for the perceptual filling in of the blind spot. (A) Cortical representation of the blind spot of the right eye when both eyes are open. The blind spot, clearly visible by the empty notch at the level of the LGN, is completely obscured in V1 due to input from the left eye. (B) Cortical representation of the same area, when the left eye is closed. Here the area corresponding to the blind spot is “filled in” at the level of V1 by feedback from neighboring layers 2/3 onto layer 6. In both cases, the blind spot is not perceived. (From Komatsu, H. (2011). Bridging gaps at V1: Neural responses for filling-in and completion at the blind spot. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 53, 413–420.) Date of download: 12/31/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.