Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Figure Legend: From: Human grasp point selection
Advertisements

From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(8):1. doi: /8.8.1 Figure Legend:
From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
From: Rational Interpolation of Car Motions
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(13):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):10. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
From: Why the long face? The importance of vertical image structure for biological “barcodes” underlying face recognition Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):25.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9.
From: Steinkamp's Toy Can Hop 100 Times But Can't Stand Up
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Pattern matching is assessed in retinotopic coordinates
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Figure Legend: From: The color lexicon of the Somali language
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Optimum spatiotemporal receptive fields for vision in dim light
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(6):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(5):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Shape, motion, and optical cues to stiffness of elastic objects
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Efficiencies for parts and wholes in biological-motion perception Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi:10.1167/17.12.21 Figure Legend: Overlap in orientation of the trajectories created by hands and feet and in left- and right-facing stimuli across rotation conditions. Each plot shows two histograms: left-facing stimuli (solid gray) and right-facing stimuli (black outline). Each histogram depicts the number of frames in which connecting the two points (either hands or feet) creates a particular angle relative to horizontal, as represented by θ in Figure 5. The isolated-hands stimuli show progressively more overlap from no rotation to bidirectional rotation to unidirectional rotation, whereas the feet show the most overlap in the bidirectional-rotation condition and the least in the unidirectional-rotation condition. Additionally, all the frames with an orientation of 0° (horizontal) are overlapping, each stimulus set having the same number of frames that satisfy this criterion in its right- as in its left-facing version. The spike at zero and the gaps immediately adjacent are due to the limited resolution of the frames: One pixel of vertical displacement at 20 pixels of horizontal displacement corresponds to an angle of 1.24°. Each bin is 1° wide. Date of download: 1/2/2018 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2018. All rights reserved.