Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(3):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Does print size matter for reading
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
From: Experimental Application of Piezoelectric Actuator-Driven Pulsed Water Jets in Retinal Vascular Surgery Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;3(6):10. doi: /tvst
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: Temporal eye movement strategies during naturalistic viewing
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Saccadic suppression during voluntary versus reactive saccades
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: A short-term memory of multi-stable perception
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: statistical inference
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion Journal of Vision.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(10):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
From: What predicts the strength of simultaneous color contrast?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Presentation transcript:

From: Vision and touch are automatically integrated for the perception of sequences of events Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi:10.1167/6.5.2 Figure Legend: Number of perceived taps as a function of both the actual number of delivered taps and the background condition. The error bars represent across-subjects standard errors. T2, T3, and T4 dotted lines represent subjects' average perception in the tactile alone condition for two, three, and four taps, respectively. Date of download: 1/7/2018 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2018. All rights reserved.