From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Figure Legend: From: Human grasp point selection
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2012;12(12):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space Journal of Vision. 2013;13(2):8.
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):24. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Holistic integration of gaze cues in visual face and body perception: Evidence from the composite design Journal of Vision. 2017;17(1):24. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Salience of unique hues and implications for color theory
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Physiologic statokinetic dissociation is eliminated by equating static and kinetic perimetry testing procedures Journal of Vision. 2016;16(14):5.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: A moving-barber-pole illusion
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
From: Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):9.
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(12):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Optimizing wavefront-guided corrections for highly aberrated eyes in the presence of registration uncertainty Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):8. doi: /
From: Optimizing wavefront-guided corrections for highly aberrated eyes in the presence of registration uncertainty Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):8. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contour extracting networks in early extrastriate cortex
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;6(4):5. doi: /tvst Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field Journal of Vision. 2016;16(2):1. doi:10.1167/16.2.1 Figure Legend: Methodological details. (A) Retinal test locations (blue rings, 10° steps in eccentricity) with symbols indicating stimulus size (1.5° black circles, 2.5° green circles, or 3.5° red circles). (B) Vector analysis of orientation bias treats acuity as a vector with magnitude = resolution spatial frequency and direction = 2× orientation of test grating. Bias is sum of vectors divided by the sum of vector lengths. (C) Nyquist frequency for an anisotropic array is the geometric mean of the Nyquist frequencies for a pair of orthogonal stimulus orientations. For the illustrated example, the x-direction is also the radial direction of stretching. (D) Convention for specifying retinal location and stimulus orientation for the right eye uses Roman typeface for terms relating to retinal location as seen by an experimenter viewing an observer's fundus. Italic typeface signifies terms relating to stimulus orientation. Meridian is measured counterclockwise from the 0° meridian (horizontal nasal retina). Absolute fringe orientation is measured by the counterclockwise angle of the bars in the fringe relative to the horizontal. Relative fringe orientation is measured from the meridian line instead of the horizontal. Radial (i.e., meridional) fringes are parallel to the meridian line and therefore have 0° relative orientation. Tangential fringes are perpendicular to the meridian line and therefore have 90° relative orientation. Date of download: 3/1/2018 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2018. All rights reserved.