Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Introduction Relative weights can be estimated by fitting a linear model using responses from individual trials: where g is the linking function. Relative.
Advertisements

Virtual Reality as a Rehabilitative Tool for Persons with Vestibular Disorders-Preliminary Findings Whitney SL 1,2,4, Furman JM 1,2,3, Redfern MS 1,2,3,
Vestibular Rehabilitation using a Wide FOV Virtual Environment PJ Sparto, JM Furman, SL Whitney, LF Hodges, MS Redfern Sponsors Eye and Ear Foundation.
MODELING MST OPTIC FLOW RESPONSES USING RECEPTIVE FIELD SEGMENTAL INTERACTIONS Chen-Ping Yu +, William K. Page*, Roger Gaborski +, and Charles J. Duffy.
The Use of Virtual Reality for Persons with Balance Disorders Susan L. Whitney, PT, PhD, NCS, ATC University of Pittsburgh Supported by the National Institute.
The Contribution Of Central And Peripheral Vision To The Postural Sway Response Elicited By Moving Visual Environments In Healthy Children Aged 8-12 Sparto.
Reliance on Dynamic Visual Cues for Postural Control: Effect of Age is More Important than Unilateral Vestibular Hypofunction Patrick J. Sparto 1,2,3,
Postural Sway in a Virtual Environment in Patients With Unilateral Peripheral Vestibular Lesions Susan L. Whitney, PhD, PT, NCS, ATC Patrick J. Sparto,
Audio-spinal reflex response in human limb muscles
Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting 2012 Daniel Mann, Charles Chubb
4aPPa32. How Susceptibility To Noise Varies Across Speech Frequencies
SPECULAR FLOW AND THE PERCEPTION OF SURFACE REFLECTANCE
David Marchant, Evelyn Carnegie, Paul Ellison
Training Method Influences on Postural Stability
From: Nonlinearities in color coding: Compensating color appearance for the eye's spectral sensitivity Journal of Vision. 2006;6(9):12. doi: /
PERCEIVED BEAUTY OF RANDOM DENSITY PATTERNS
Visually-Induced Postural Sway in Children
Uri Hasson, Ifat Levy, Marlene Behrmann, Talma Hendler, Rafael Malach 
Contribution of spatial and temporal integration in heading perception
During Anterior-Posterior Optic Flow
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
MODELING MST OPTIC FLOW RESPONSES
11/24/2018 Sensory Re-Weighting In Human Postural Control During Moving-Scene Perturbations A. Mahboobin1, P. Loughlin1,2, Ph.D., M. Redfern3,2, Ph.D.,
Volume 52, Issue 6, Pages (December 2006)
Gijsbert Stoet, Lawrence H Snyder  Neuron 
Illusory Jitter Perceived at the Frequency of Alpha Oscillations
Michael S Beauchamp, Kathryn E Lee, Brenna D Argall, Alex Martin 
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
MODELING MST OPTIC FLOW RESPONSES
Sparto PJ, Furman JM, Jacobson JL, Whitney SL, Hodges LF, Redfern MS
VISUAL DEPENDENCE IN POSTURAL CONTROL AND SPATIAL ORIENTATION Massimo Cenciarini1, Patrick J. Loughlin1,2, Mark S. Redfern1,3, Patrick J. Sparto1,3 Depts.
Retinal Representation of the Elementary Visual Signal
Volume 51, Issue 1, Pages (July 2006)
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Volume 74, Issue 5, Pages (June 2012)
Volume 79, Issue 4, Pages (August 2013)
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
James A. Strother, Aljoscha Nern, Michael B. Reiser  Current Biology 
A Map for Horizontal Disparity in Monkey V2
Jianing Yu, David Ferster  Neuron 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Michael S Beauchamp, Kathryn E Lee, Brenna D Argall, Alex Martin 
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Ilan Lampl, Iva Reichova, David Ferster  Neuron 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Stability of Cortical Responses and the Statistics of Natural Scenes
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Stimulus display and illustrations of spectral and spatial patterns of SSVEP amplitudes. Stimulus display and illustrations of spectral and spatial patterns.
Gijsbert Stoet, Lawrence H Snyder  Neuron 
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Color Signals in Human Motion-Selective Cortex
Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision
Taosheng Liu, Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco  Neuron 
Pairing-Induced Changes of Orientation Maps in Cat Visual Cortex
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Presentation transcript:

Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment The Role of Central and Peripheral Optic Flow in the Control of Upright Posture Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment Jeff G. Jasko1, Patrick J. Sparto 1,2,3, Mark S. Redfern1,3, and Patrick J. Loughlin1,4 Departments of Bioengineering1, Physical Therapy2, Otolaryngology3, and Electrical Engineering4 University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Table 1. Optic flow conditions. ABSTRACT Several theories have been developed in an attempt to characterize the functional roles of central and peripheral vision in maintaining postural equilibrium (Bardy et al. 1999). While some findings suggest that peripheral vision dominates postural control, others suggest that central vision is equally important in the perception of self-motion. Given these diverging results, further research is needed to clarify this issue. The goal of this study was to investigate the influence of optic flow on upright posture when patterns in the central and peripheral fields of view (FOVs) move simultaneously but at different sinusoidal frequencies. Twenty healthy subjects (9 males, 11 females; ages 21-30) participated in the experiment. They were surrounded by an image encompassing 180 x 70 (horiz. x vert.) FOV. The central stimulus was a pattern of alternating black-and-white concentric rings, and the peripheral stimulus consisted of black-and-white checkers. There were two visual combinations: in one, the central optic flow stimulus moved at 0.1 Hz while the peripheral optic flow moved at 0.25 Hz; in the other, the frequencies were reversed. The peak-to-peak amplitude of all stimuli was 16 cm, in the anterior-posterior direction. In addition, there were 2 support-surface conditions: fixed or sway-referenced. Head position was recorded during the 90-second trials. A statistical test determined whether there was a significant response at either stimulus frequency. Subjects who had significant responses during ¾ of the trials were considered “consistent responders”. Root-Mean-Square (RMS) sway of the responders’ data was calculated at both stimulus frequencies using a bandpass filter, and then normalized to their quiet-stance sway. Within each visual/platform condition, the normalized sway at the peripheral frequency was compared to the normalized sway at the central frequency using paired t-tests. There were more significant responses to the peripheral optic flow stimulus (60/80) than there were to the central optic flow (18/80). Fourteen subjects were classified as responders. In 3 of the 4 visual/platform conditions, the normalized RMS sway at the peripheral frequency was significantly greater than the normalized sway at the central frequency. This suggests that the postural system is more sensitive to optic flow in the peripheral FOV, regardless of frequency. Figure 1. Visual environment encompassed 180° X 70° (horiz. X vert.) of the subjects’ field of view. 2 Platform Conditions: Fixed and Sway-Referenced Recorded head movement with Polhemus Fastrak Duration of each trial: 90 seconds 20 healthy subjects (mean age: 24  3 years) Central and peripheral regions of the visual environment moved simultaneously at different frequencies with a peak-to-peak amplitude of 16 cm, in the anterior-posterior direction. PURPOSE To investigate the influence of optic flow on upright posture when patterns in the central and peripheral fields of view (FOVs) move simultaneously but at different sinusoidal frequencies. METHODS RESULTS Table 2. The number of significant responses to the central and peripheral stimulus frequencies during each of the visual/platform conditions, as well as the p-values from the 2 analysis. Includes only the consistent responders (n = 14). If responses were observed at both stimulus frequencies in one trial, a response was tabulated for each stimulus region. The number of responses to the periphery was significantly greater than the number of responses to the center, regardless of stimulus frequency. There were 17 responses to the central stimulus. 15 of these responses were concurrent with responses to the periphery. In 53 out of 56 trials from this subgroup (consistent responders), there was a significant response to at least one of the stimulus frequencies. a b Figure 2. Time series (a) and power spectrum (b) of a subject who responded significantly to both stimulus frequencies during the Sway-Referenced Platform, Central-0.25 Hz/Peripheral-0.1 Hz condition. Within subplot a, the blue trace is the subject’s head motion while the red trace is the sum of the motion of the two stimulus regions, scaled to 1/8 its actual amplitude. Peripheral 0.1 Hz Central 0.25 Hz Low-Frequency Sway During Quiet Stance is a Confounding Factor. Sway obtained during moving-scene trials is a superposition of the response at the stimulus frequencies and quiet-stance (QS) sway (Figure 3). Therefore, absolute RMS sway includes the subjects’ QS sway and their response to the stimulus. The mean QS sway at 0.1 Hz was greater than the QS sway at 0.25 Hz. In order to determine how much the subjects reacted to the stimulus, we normalized their RMS values by subtracting out the QS component at each of the stimulus frequencies (Figure 4). Figure 3. Mean power spectra of the consistent responders’ sway during quiet stance and during the two stimulus conditions, sway-referenced platform only. Figure 4. Comparison of the consistent responders’ normalized RMS sway at the two stimulus frequencies within each visual/platform condition. The four pairings correspond to the four conditions. Within each pairing, the left and right bars represent the average ( SEM) normalized sway at the central stimulus and peripheral frequencies, respectively. The solid bars refer to 0.1 Hz, and the textured bars, to 0.25 Hz. Significant differences were observed in the bracketed pairs marked with asterisks (*). Within 3 of the 4 visual/platform conditions, the normalized sway at the peripheral frequency was significantly greater than the normalized sway at the central frequency (Figure 4). CONCLUSIONS The peripheral stimulus caused significantly more sway than the central stimulus. This suggests that the postural system is more sensitive to anterior-posterior optic flow in the peripheral FOV, regardless of stimulus frequency. 2 test showed a greater number of significant responses to the peripheral stimulus. Within 3 of the 4 visual/platform conditions, the absolute and normalized sway at the peripheral frequency was significantly greater than the sway at the central frequency. These findings have implications for the use of virtual environments, showing that head-mounted displays with a limited central FOV do not provide the visual information most important for postural control. Furthermore, the results suggest that rehabilitation therapists should incorporate peripheral movement cues in treatment to enhance balance retraining. Acknowledgements This research was supported by grants from NIH/NIA-1K25 AG01049, NIH/NIDCD-DC02490, and the Eye and Ear Foundation. References Bardy, B.G. et al. (1999). Perception & Psychophysics. 61, 1356-68. Percival, D.B. (1994). `Spectral Analysis of Univariate and Bivariate Time Series,' in Statistical Methods for Physical Science. Academic Press. Figure 5. Comparison of the consistent responders’ normalized sway at one stimulus frequency between the two visual conditions. The normalized sway at a given frequency was also compared between the two frequency combinations (e.g., Center-0.1 Hz vs. Periphery-0.1 Hz), and in all four cases, the response to a frequency presented in the periphery was significantly greater than when the same frequency was presented in the center (Figure 5). Quiet Stance Stimulus Region Stimulus 1 Stimulus 2 Central Target 0.1 Hz 0.25 Hz Peripheral Checkers