Corrugator and startle responses predict memory for emotional pictures: differential effects by emotion category Michelle M. Wirth, Roxanne M. Monticelli,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Event-related potentials (ERPs) have been used in past research to study the correlates and consequences of alcohol use (Porjesz et al., 2005). In particular,
Advertisements

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY U N I V E R S I T Y O F C O P E N H A G E N Suppression of neutral but not emotional words Background Anderson & Green (2001)
Group 3-Youngjin Kang, Alyssa Nolde, Antoinette Sellers, Zhiheng Zhou
RESULTSINTRODUCTION Sexual desire, as the cognitive component of sexual arousal, is thought to be influenced by attention directed, and emotional response,
C RESULTS: SNR of visual gamma oscillations Robust visual gamma responses were observed in 18 of the 27 subjects analysed Figure 4: A) SNR plotted as a.
Pre-processing for EEG and MEG
Experiment 2: MEG Study Materials and Methods: 11 right-handed subjects with 20:20 vision were run. 3 subjects’ data was discarded because of poor performance.
Phonetic Similarity Effects in Masked Priming Marja-Liisa Mailend 1, Edwin Maas 1, & Kenneth I. Forster 2 1 Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing.
Hunger and satiety in anorexia nervosa: fMRI during cognitive processing of food pictures Santel et al., 2006 Maria Zuluaga Bsc Psychology 14’
Altering cognitive and brain states through cortical entrainment: A pilot study of binaural beats in visual target detection using dense-array EEG Nicholas.
Theta-Coupled Periodic Replay in Working Memory Lluís Fuentemilla, Will D Penny, Nathan Cashdollar, Nico Bunzeck, Emrah Düzel Current Biology, 2010,20(7):
Group 4 Alicia Iafonaro Anthony Correa Baoyu Wang Isaac Del Rio Measuring Emotion: Behavior, Feeling, and Physiology.
Pavlovian, Observational and Instructed Fear Learning: Emotional Responses to Unmasked and Masked Stimuli Andreas Olsson, Kristen Stedenfeld & Elizabeth.
INTRODUCTION AIMS AND PREDICTIONS METHODS Participants: 18 children (9-10; M = 10). 38 young adults (20-30; M = 24) 26 older adults (65-85; M = 72) EEG.
Functional Brain Signal Processing: EEG & fMRI Lesson 4
ABSTRACT Two experiments comparing the efficiency of four different retention strategies for vocabulary-based learning were examined on a total 295 undergraduate.
Spatial Visualization Training Using Interactive Animations Cheryl A. Cohen Mary Hegarty University of California, Santa Barbara Department of Psychology.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Introduction Can you read the following paragraph? Can we derive meaning from words even if they are distorted by intermixing words with numbers? Perea,
The Role of Mixed Emotional States in Predicting Men’s and Women’s Subjective and Physiological Sexual Responses to Erotic Stimuli Peterson, Z. D. 1 and.
Introduction  Conway 1 proposes there are two types of autobiographical event memories (AMs):  Unique, specific events  Repeated, general events  These.
Disrupting face biases in visual attention Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling Introduction Method.
The role of synchronous gamma-band activity in schizophrenia Jakramate 2009 / 01 / 14.
Experiment 2: N = Factor Repeated Measures ANCOVA  Span Factor Score Covariate: Shortened Complex Span Tasks. 8  Conditions: Negative-Arousing.
An ERP investigation of response inhibition in adults with DCD Elisabeth Hill Duncan Brown José van Velzen.
The Effect of Retro-Cueing on an ERP Marker of VSTM Maintenance Alexandra M Murray, Bo-Cheng Kuo, Mark G Stokes, Anna C Nobre Brain & Cognition Laboratory,
N=52 self-reported Christian undergraduates were recruited to the lab and told they would play a coin flipping game (i.e., Long et al., 2012) against another.
Norming Study Mechanisms of Emotion Regulation: The Role of Attentional Control Lindsey R. Wallace, M. A. & Elisabeth J. Ploran, Ph.D. Department of Psychology,
Sascha Schneider Steve Nebel Günter Daniel Rey
The Role of Arousal in Mood Mediation: A Closer Look at Mood Congruent Memory Ben Fox, Janna Kost, Sabette Thompson, Michelle Fung, Dawn Macaulay, Eric.
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Concepts of Habituation and Sensitization
Feature Binding: Not Quite So Pre-attentive Erin Buchanan and M
The Oxford Vocal (OxVoc) Sounds Database:
Difference in Mls poured between the subject and the researcher
Behavioral Results SUMMARY
Variations in Oscillatory Power During Rule Switching
Attention Components and Creative Potential: An ERP Exploration
Verifiability and Action verb Processing: An ERP Investigation
Neurofeedback of beta frequencies:
US location switch alone
A High-Density EEG investigation of the Misinformation Effect: Differentiating between True and False Memories John E. Kiat & Robert F. Belli Department.
Marc E. Lavoie Emma Glaser, Adrianna Mendrek
Meghan Brzinski and David Havas, Ph.D.
Stewart Shankman, Ph.D. Professor University of Illinois at Chicago
Investigating the Negativity Bias Using the Affective Go/No Go and ERPs Jason Krompinger, Jason S. Moser, & Robert F. Simons Department of Psychology,
Alison Burros, Nathan Herdener, & Mei-Ching Lien
The Effects of Musical Mood and Musical Arousal on Visual Attention
MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre
Jamie Cummins Bryan Roche Aoife Cartwright Maynooth University
Social context influence emotional language comprehension
Elana G. Brubaker, Allison L. Jahn, Heather C. Abercrombie, Richard J
Elizabeth A. Phelps, Joseph E. LeDoux  Neuron 
Clemens Kirschbaumc, and Heather C. Abercrombiea
Recall test for previously encoded words
Word Imagery Effects on Explicit and Implicit Memory
The Effects of Exogenously-Elevated Cortisol in Inhibiting Habituation to Emotionally Arousing Contexts Sean M. Scherer Introductory Biology 152, Dept.
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Threat signals of pain modulate defensive responses in observers: development of an experimental paradigm Goubert, L.1*, PhD, Caes, L. 1, MSc, Uzieblo,
The Role of Arousal in Mood Mediation: A Closer Look at Mood Congruent Memory Eric Eich 1/17/2019.
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Perirhinal-Hippocampal Connectivity during Reactivation Is a Marker for Object-Based Memory Consolidation  Kaia L. Vilberg, Lila Davachi  Neuron  Volume.
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Gamma and Beta Bursts Underlie Working Memory
Machine Learning for Visual Scene Classification with EEG Data
Athanassios G Siapas, Matthew A Wilson  Neuron 
Wallis, JD Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute UC, Berkeley
Frontal Beta Brainwave
Anthony D. Wagner, Anat Maril, and Daniel L. Schacter
Presentation transcript:

Corrugator and startle responses predict memory for emotional pictures: differential effects by emotion category Michelle M. Wirth, Roxanne M. Monticelli, Daren C. Jackson, Anand Lakshmanan, and Heather C. Abercrombie Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin - Madison DISCUSSION Our findings show that stimulus valence affects the relationship between startle and memory differently than between corrugator and memory. In particular, pleasant stimuli that are later remembered elicit more startle activity but less corrugator activity (pleasant picture effects confirmed by post-hoc comparisons.) Corrugator and startle have been shown to track negative affect and valence properties of stimuli. (Bradley et al., 2001) Emotional enhancement of memory seems to depend on arousal properties of stimuli. However, our data suggest that emotional enhancement of memory involves a combination of arousal and valence. Prior evidence implicates the amygdala in both emotion-potentiated startle (Davis, 1986) and emotional enhancement of memory / emotional learning (LeDoux, 1992). Our data are suggestive of overlapping but distinct neural systems underlying enhancement of memory by positive / pleasant vs. negative / unpleasant stimuli. Brain imaging and animal work is needed to help elucidate this hypothesis. METHODS, cont. RESULTS BACKGROUND Emotional stimuli and events are remembered better than neutral stimuli / events. In particular, it has been found that emotionally arousing stimuli are remembered better than non-arousing stimuli. (Bradley et al., 1992) Corrugator (frown) muscle activity and startle (eyeblink reflex) responses are responsive to emotional state or emotional content of a stimulus. (Bradley et al., 2001) Corrugator and startle responses tend to be intensified by negative affect and unpleasant stimuli, and lessened by positive affect / pleasant stimuli. In this study, we investigated the relationship between corrugator and startle responses to emotional visual stimuli and participants’ later memory for those stimuli. Subjects remembered more emotional than neutral words, replicating past literature (also more unpleasant than pleasant words). Picture valence affected startle and corrugator responses, also replicating past literature. After encoding, participants performed a speech stressor task to investigate other hypotheses (see Abercrombie et al., 2006). RETRIEVAL SESSION occurred 2 days after the first (“encoding”) session. Participants first performed a free recall test, followed by recognition tests (with 63 targets and 63 equivalent distractor stimuli.) 4 participants excluded for incomplete free recall data and 4 for insufficient valid startle trials (see below), leaving a sample of 44. Psychophysiology Psychophysiology data collection modeled after Jackson et al., 2000. Miniature Ag/AgCl electrodes placed for collection of EMG data from corrugator supercilii and orbicularis oculi according to Tassinary and Cacioppo (2000). Data amplified using a gain of 10,000, highpass filter 1 Hz, sampling rate 1000 Hz. Startle: lowpass filter 800 Hz; corrugator: lowpass filter 400 Hz. Startle eyeblink data integrated and rectified using a Coulbourn S76-01 contour-following integrator with a time constant of 20 ms. Startle trials were excluded if they contained excessive noise, or if the onset of the eyeblink began less than 15 ms after the startle probe. Only participants who had at least two trials free from excessive noise per probe time per picture valence were included in analyses. (4 participants were excluded.) Startle blink magnitudes (mV) = (max peak integrated EMG btwn 20 – 120 ms from probe onset) minus (amount of integrated EMG at reflex onset). Noise-free trials with no perceptible eyeblink reflex were given a magnitude of zero. Blink magnitudes Z-transformed within-subjects. Startle blink magnitudes were collapsed over probe time within subjects for each cell in the present analyses (e.g., for remembered pleasant pictures). Corrugator data were scored for artifact. A fast Hartley transform (FHT) was performed on all artifact-free 1.024-s chunks of data (extracted through overlapping Hamming windows) to derive estimates of spectral power density (mV2) in the 45–200-Hz frequency band. These values were log-transformed to normalize the data. Corrugator data divided into epochs: baseline (containing activity averaged over 2 seconds during ITI prior to picture onset) and early-picture (containing activity averaged over 3 seconds after picture onset). Corrugator reactivity to pictures was calculated by subtracting baseline score from early-picture score for a given trial. For startle, there was an overall effect of memory: Remembered pictures were associated with more startle activity than forgotten pictures, across valences. (Main effect of memory, F(1,43)=8.010 p=0.007) METHODS 61 male participants recruited to study effects of emotion-related physiology on memory. 9 excluded due to failing to show up for memory session or experimenter error. Final samples 44 and 19 (see below). PROCEDURE Participants completed 2 sessions (“encoding” and “retrieval”). ENCODING SESSION: participants viewed pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant IAPS pictures (21 of each emotion category, chosen using normative ratings). Pictures were displayed for 6 seconds with an ITI of 17 seconds. Startle probes (50 ms blasts of 95 decibel white noise) were presented through headphones at 4.5, 7, or 8 seconds into the trial (18 trials each) or not at all (9 trials). Corrugator EMG and blink (orbicularis) EMG were recorded with mini-electrodes throughout picture viewing (encoding). Participants were not informed that there would be a memory test later. REFERENCES Abercrombie, H. C., Speck, N. S., & Monticelli, R. M. (2006). Endogenous cortisol elevations are related to memory facilitation only in individuals who are emotionally aroused. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(2), 187-196. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 18(2), 379-390. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276-298. Davis, M. (1986). Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100(6), 814-824. Jackson, D. C., Malmstadt, J. R., Larson, C. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Suppression and enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 37(4), 515-522. LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Brain mechanisms of emotion and emotional learning. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 2(2), 191-197. Tassinary, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). The skeletomotor system: Surface electromyography. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. For corrugator, there was an interaction between valence and memory (F(2,86)=4.075, p=0.020). Unpleasant pictures that were later remembered elicited MORE corrugator response than forgotten unpleasant pictures. Pleasant pictures that were later remembered elicited LESS corrugator response than forgotten pleasant pictures. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was made possible in part by institutional start-up funds to Heather Abercrombie (University of Wisconsin), and NIMH Institutional training grant number T32MH018931 from the National Institute of Mental Health (the Emotion Research Training Grant, which supports Michelle Wirth.) We are grateful to Nicole Speck, Natasha Shallow, Ashley Lienhardt, and Marie Kay for assistance with data collection. We also would like to thank Richard Davidson and Ned Kalin for advice and support. These effects were also found for recognition memory, in an overlapping but distinct sample (N=19).