Mesopelagic Cephalopods Switch between Transparency and Pigmentation to Optimize Camouflage in the Deep  Sarah Zylinski, Sönke Johnsen  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Advertisements

Harold A. Burgess, Hannah Schoch, Michael Granato  Current Biology 
Circular Polarization Vision in a Stomatopod Crustacean
Vision and the light environment
Liangtang Chang, Qin Fang, Shikun Zhang, Mu-ming Poo, Neng Gong 
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Cephalopod dynamic camouflage
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Volume 17, Issue 10, Pages (May 2007)
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
A Unique Advantage for Giant Eyes in Giant Squid
Andres Laan, Tamar Gutnick, Michael J. Kuba, Gilles Laurent 
Shaul Druckmann, Dmitri B. Chklovskii  Current Biology 
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages e3 (October 2017)
Attention Robustly Gates a Closed-Loop Touch Reflex
Kinematic Diversity in Rorqual Whale Feeding Mechanisms
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Reconstitution of Amoeboid Motility In Vitro Identifies a Motor-Independent Mechanism for Cell Body Retraction  Katsuya Shimabukuro, Naoki Noda, Murray.
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Box Jellyfish Use Terrestrial Visual Cues for Navigation
Volume 15, Issue 24, Pages (December 2005)
Pupil Size Coupling to Cortical States Protects the Stability of Deep Sleep via Parasympathetic Modulation  Özge Yüzgeç, Mario Prsa, Robert Zimmermann,
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 26, Issue 16, Pages R752-R754 (August 2016)
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Miklós Bech, Uwe Homberg, Keram Pfeiffer  Current Biology 
A Map for Horizontal Disparity in Monkey V2
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Rapid Innate Defensive Responses of Mice to Looming Visual Stimuli
Attention Reduces Spatial Uncertainty in Human Ventral Temporal Cortex
Volume 25, Issue 11, Pages (June 2015)
Directional Selectivity Is Formed at Multiple Levels by Laterally Offset Inhibition in the Rabbit Retina  Shelley I. Fried, Thomas A. Mu¨nch, Frank S.
Reconstitution of Amoeboid Motility In Vitro Identifies a Motor-Independent Mechanism for Cell Body Retraction  Katsuya Shimabukuro, Naoki Noda, Murray.
Cephalopod Behaviour: Skin Flicks
Non-overlapping Neural Networks in Hydra vulgaris
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Neural Circuit Components of the Drosophila OFF Motion Vision Pathway
Nonsomatotopic Organization of the Higher Motor Centers in Octopus
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Steffan Kennett, Marisa Taylor-Clarke, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Drew Friedmann, Adam Hoagland, Shai Berlin, Ehud Y. Isacoff 
Vision and lack of vision in the ocean
MT Neurons Combine Visual Motion with a Smooth Eye Movement Signal to Code Depth-Sign from Motion Parallax  Jacob W. Nadler, Mark Nawrot, Dora E. Angelaki,
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Bettina Schnell, Ivo G. Ros, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Kwoon Y. Wong, Felice A. Dunn, David M. Berson  Neuron 
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Resolution of Uncertainty in Prefrontal Cortex
Marie P. Suver, Akira Mamiya, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Supratim Ray, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Vision and the light environment
Steven C. Leiser, Karen A. Moxon  Neuron 
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
James N. Ingram, Ian S. Howard, J. Randall Flanagan, Daniel M. Wolpert 
Liangtang Chang, Qin Fang, Shikun Zhang, Mu-ming Poo, Neng Gong 
Volume 15, Issue 24, Pages (December 2005)
Presentation transcript:

Mesopelagic Cephalopods Switch between Transparency and Pigmentation to Optimize Camouflage in the Deep  Sarah Zylinski, Sönke Johnsen  Current Biology  Volume 21, Issue 22, Pages 1937-1941 (November 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.014 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Cartoon Summary of the Strengths and Weaknesses of Transparency and Pigmentation as Camouflage Strategies in the Mesopelagic In (A) and (B), hypothetical transparent and pigmented animals are viewed against the downwelling ambient light by Argyropelecus, a deep-sea hatchet fish with its characteristic upward-orientated eyes suited to the task, and under the biological searchlights of the myctophid Diaphus, known as the headlight fish. (A) The prevalence of transparency in the upper mesopelagic can be explained as a close-to-perfect solution to the dilemma faced by pelagic organisms of having nowhere to hide, being effective from all viewing angles (body parts that cannot be made transparent, such as guts and eyes, are often mirrored or hidden via counterilluminating photophores) [2]. Imperfections in tissue transparency result in unavoidable internal light scattering which, under diffuse light, has a minor effect on the animal's visibility. However, under directed light, such as that emitted from the photophores thought to function as biological “searchlights” possessed by predators (or indeed by any other close-by source of bioluminescence) [3–7], the scattered light returning to a viewer will be much brighter than the background, leaving the animal at risk of predation [2, 4]. (B) At greater depths, the ubiquity of transparency shifts to one of red or black pigmentation [9, 10], suggesting that another form of visual detection is at play. Minimum reflectance for measured pigmented deep-pelagic animals occurs at ∼480–490 nm [12], coinciding with the wavelengths of most known bioluminescent emission peaks and visual pigment absorption maxima of a majority of deep-sea fish and invertebrate species measured [3, 11, 13, 14]. This will enable them to avoid detection under biological searchlights and other nearby sources of bioluminescence, but they will be susceptible to detection via their silhouettes under downwelling light, which is often orders of magnitude brighter than the upwelling light. (C) This paper reports on findings from two species of cephalopod that suggest they switch between transparency and pigmentation to be optimally camouflaged under both ambient downwelling light and under directed bioluminescence. This switch can occur rapidly in both directions via the expansion and contraction of intradermal chromatophores, allowing predator-specific camouflage to be maintained. Current Biology 2011 21, 1937-1941DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Results of Behavioral Experiments with Japetella heathi (A) The same individual J. heathi octopus in transparent mode (left) and pigmented mode (right). Expansion of chromatophores was elicited by a tactile stimulus (a blunt needle pressed against arms). Photographs were taken within 5 s of each other. (B) Responses of a single J. heathi to directed blue light. Yellow boxes and icon indicate onset and cessation of individual lighting “bouts,” consisting of a flashing blue light at one flash per second. Most bouts lasted for 3 s and therefore subjected the animal to three flashes. Chromatophores can be seen to expand seconds after initial exposure. After continued exposure, the animal ceased reacting to the light with chromatophore responses and instead displayed evasive behavior such as swimming away from the light source and retraction of the head into the mantle. See Movie S1 for footage. (C) Responses of J. heathi to four different stimuli. Objects passed in front of the animals and shadows passed overhead failed to evoke a significant increase in chromatophore expression, although the animals could be seen to track the stimulus with eye movements. A tactile stimulus (touching the arms with a blunt needle) resulted in the rapid expansion of chromatophores and evasive behavior (retraction of the head into the mantle cavity). By comparison, directed blue light resulted in a rapid and strong expression of chromatophores (data obtained in a separate experiment). White circles = prestimulus, and black circles = poststimulus. Error bars show standard deviation from mean. P relates to paired t tests between pre- and poststimulus chromatophore cover. ns = not significant with p > 0.05; ∗∗∗ = significant with p < 0.001. No test for significance is included for blue light treatment because of the low n. Current Biology 2011 21, 1937-1941DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Results and Details of Behavioral Experiments with Onychoteuthis banksii (A) Images of O. banksii taken from video footage showing (L) the chromatophores contracted and the animal transparent under ambient light conditions and (R) with the chromatophores expanded and the animal pigmented when subjected to directed light. See Movie S2 for footage. (B) Chromatophore responses of two individual O. banksii to three bouts of directed light. The blue line indicates response to blue light, and the red line indicates response to red light (see C). Yellow boxes with symbols show the onset and offset of each lighting bout. (C) Irradiance of lights used in experiments, blue line for blue filter with white LED (450 nm peak emission 20 nm full-width at half maximum intensity) and red line for red filter with white LED (600 nm cut-on). Black line shows a generalized sensitivity curve for a single visual pigment with a peak sensitivity at 480 nm typical of deep-sea vision [11] (y axis arbitrary). Current Biology 2011 21, 1937-1941DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Reflectance Measurements and Data (A) Setup for measuring reflectance from Japetella showing reflectance probe connected to light source and spectrometer. Inset shows arrangement of six illuminant fibers surrounding a single measurement fiber. Measurements were taken underwater, from five positions on the mantle forward of the gut. The reflectance probe was positioned 90° relative to the skin and held at a constant angle and distance by a clamp. The animals were placed in a small tank in limited water depth to restrict movement. (B) Measured reflectance from live Japetella skin. Black line shows reflectance from transparent form, with chromatophores contracted. Gray line shows reflectance from pigmented form, with chromatophores expanded. Means from five areas measured on four individuals (mantle length of ∼50 mm). Dark and light shaded areas show standard deviation from means. Current Biology 2011 21, 1937-1941DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions